Skip to main content

Uncovering heterogeneous cognitive trajectories in mild cognitive impairment: a data-driven approach

Abstract

Background

Given the complex and progressive nature of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), the ability to delineate and understand the heterogeneous cognitive trajectories is crucial for developing personalized medicine and informing trial design. The primary goals of this study were to examine whether different cognitive trajectories can be identified within subjects with MCI and, if present, to characterize each trajectory in relation to changes in all major Alzheimer’s disease (AD) biomarkers over time.

Methods

Individuals with a diagnosis of MCI at the first visit and ≥ 1 follow-up cognitive assessment were selected from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative database (n = 936; age 73 ± 8; 40% female; 16 ± 3 years of education; 50% APOE4 carriers). Based on the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive Subscale-13 (ADAS-Cog-13) total scores from baseline up to 5 years follow-up, a non-parametric k-means longitudinal clustering method was performed to obtain clusters of individuals with similar patterns of cognitive decline. We further conducted a series of linear mixed-effects models to study the associations of cluster membership with longitudinal changes in other cognitive measures, neurodegeneration, and in vivo AD pathologies.

Results

Four distinct cognitive trajectories emerged. Cluster 1 consisted of 255 individuals (27%) with a nearly non-existent rate of change in the ADAS-Cog-13 over 5 years of follow-up and a healthy-looking biomarker profile. Individuals in the cluster 2 (n = 336, 35%) and 3 (n = 240, 26%) groups showed relatively mild and moderate cognitive decline trajectories, respectively. Cluster 4, comprising about 11% of our study sample (n = 105), exhibited an aggressive cognitive decline trajectory and was characterized by a pronouncedly abnormal biomarker profile.

Conclusions

Individuals with MCI show substantial heterogeneity in cognitive decline. Our findings may potentially contribute to improved trial design and patient stratification.

Background

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is often thought of as a transitional stage between normal cognitive aging and dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [1, 2]. However, MCI is linked to substantial biological heterogeneity [3, 4]. Although the annual rate of conversion from MCI to AD is set at approximately 10–12% [5,6,7], not all who have a diagnosis of MCI demonstrate progressive decline, and many exhibit differential clinical outcomes, including remaining at the MCI level or reverting to cognitively normal (CN) state [8, 9]. In spite of its ubiquity, the heterogeneity of MCI regarding cognitive trajectories and progression to AD is currently not well understood, hindering further progress in clinical practice and research.

To date, there are no effective therapies for the disease, and most clinical trials, aimed to slow down or halt the conversion from MCI to dementia, have so far failed [10]. One possible reason for widespread failures of therapeutic development for the disease may be due to neglecting the heterogeneous nature of MCI and treating all individuals as if they were the same. In line with this notion, a recent simulation study has shown that individuals with early AD demonstrated substantially varying rates of cognitive decline, even when randomization procedures were applied at the start of the study [11]. For instance, one potential way to facilitate the development of effective therapies is to identify and remove subgroups of MCI individuals who have relatively normal cognitive performance and a low rate of progression to AD [12]. The authors suggest that the inclusion of subjects with “disease-free” MCI may attenuate the potential beneficial effects of treatment.

Several previous studies have utilized subtyping approaches to sort out the heterogeneity of MCI in a non-biased manner [4, 13,14,15,16,17,18,19]. With regard to delineating cognitive subtypes, several investigators have utilized data-driven and specifically cluster-analytic approaches based on cross-sectional neuropsychological assessment data. Delano-Wood was one of the first to provide evidence that distinct subgroups of MCI can be empirically derived based on cognitive data using a clinical sample of 70 [20]. Subsequent studies applying empirical methods to neuropsychological test scores have identified multiple MCI subgroups in clinic-based [21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28], community-based [29, 30], and clinical trial [12] samples. However, given the progressive nature of cognitive aging or AD, considering the longitudinal progression and not just a snapshot of the current state may provide a more comprehensive picture of the prodrome stage of the disease. In Xie et al.’s [31] study, group-based trajectory modeling (GBTM) has been performed to identify 5 different longitudinal cognitive trajectories on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score in subjects with MCI. In three other studies, by Lee et al. [32], Kim et al. [33], and Kim et al. [34], the GBTM method was applied to discover cognitive trajectories according to Clinical Dementia Rating Sum of Boxes (CDR-SB) in subjects with MCI. The first study by Lee et al. identified two cognitive trajectories (fast decliners and slow decliners), while the other two investigations discovered three cognitive trajectories (stable, slow decliners, and faster decliners). Despite the prominent role of the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive Subscale 13 (ADAS-Cog-13) in evaluating the efficacy of antidementia medications [35], only one study has attempted to delineate the heterogeneity of cognitive trajectories of ADAS-Cog-13 among 238 participants with amyloid-positive MCI [36]. The identified three clusters showed different cognitive trajectories over time. However, no studies have attempted to uncover MCI heterogeneity based on longitudinal ADAS-Cog-13 scores in both amyloid-positive and amyloid-negative subjects. The inclusion of all MCI subjects regardless of amyloid status is clinically relevant, particularly in a clinical trial targeting other pathobiological pathways other than the amyloid pathway. Additionally, further validation of identified cognitive trajectories using other neuropsychological tests, neurodegeneration, in vivo AD pathologies, and clinical progression may enhance the robustness of the resulting clusters.

Thus, by applying a data-driven, longitudinal clustering analysis approach, we investigated whether distinct cognitive trajectories could be derived within the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) MCI cohort and, if present, assessed the associations of trajectory membership with longitudinal changes in all major AD biomarkers.

Methods

ADNI database

Data used in the present study were obtained from the ADNI database. The ADNI study was launched in 2003, and its primary goal has been to measure the clinical progression of MCI and early AD by utilizing numerous markers, such as clinical, neuropsychological, and imaging assessments. Recruitment procedures for the ADNI cohort have been described at the following website: www.loni.usc.edu/ADNI, and the ADNI eligibility criteria can be found at the following website: www.adni-info.org/Scientists/ADNIStudyProcedures.html.

Participants

Our study focuses on the 936 individuals diagnosed with amnestic MCI at baseline and at least one follow-up assessment (with ADAS-Cog-13 administration) in the next 5 years. These follow-up time points were included regardless of the diagnostic status of the subject at these visits. Follow-up visits beyond 5 years after baseline were not included in the analyses. MCI diagnosis was assigned to an individual if he/she met the following criteria: memory complaint, memory impairment as verified by the Logical Memory II subscale (delayed paragraph recall) from the Wechsler Memory Scale-revised (WMS-R), MMSE [37] score of between 24 to 30 (inclusive), CDR [38] score of 0.5, essentially preserved activities of daily living, and absence of AD or dementia. These ADNI MCI criteria are largely consistent with commonly used criteria in clinical trials, such as the Peterson criteria [6].

Each ADNI participant or authorized representative provided written informed consent and the institutional review board of each participating ADNI site approved the ADNI study. This project was also submitted for review to the institutional review board of Wenzhou Seventh People’s Hospital. However, given that this study did not involve contact with human subjects and used de-identified data, the institutional review board of Wenzhou Seventh People’s Hospital determined this study did not require review.

Neuropsychological tests

ADNI participants underwent a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological assessments during visits. Four primary cognitive tests were selected in this analysis. We included the ADAS-Cog-13 [39], which examines 13 aspects of cognitive function (range: 0–85, higher scores represent more severe cognitive impairment). MMSE, one of the most popular brief screening cognitive tests, was included as a measure of global cognition. The ADNI Memory composite score was derived from MMSE, logical memory, ADAS-Cog (3 versions), and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (2 versions) [40]. The ADNI executive function composite score was developed from Clock Drawing, Digit Span Backwards, Category Fluency, Trails A and B, and Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised Digit Symbol Substitution. Both the ADNI Memory and Executive function composite scores have been validated in previously published studies [41, 42].

Structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) measures

The procedure for MRI acquisition has been described previously [43]. Temporal lobe atrophy, particularly in the hippocampal formation and entorhinal cortex, has been considered to be the biological alteration most proximal to the onset of cognitive impairment [44]. Additionally, ventricular enlargement is thought to be a valid measure of clinical progression in individuals with MCI or AD [45]. In this study, we thus focus on these three structural MRI markers. To adjust sex differences in head size, three structural MRI markers were calculated using the following formulas:

$$Adjusted\;hippocampal\;volume\;(aHV)\;=\;hippocampal/intracranial\;volume\;\times\;10^3$$
$$Adjusted\;entorhinal\;cortex\;volume\;(aEV)\;=\;entorhinal\;cortex/intracranial\;volume\;\times\;10^3$$
$$Adjusted\;ventricular\;volume\;(aVV)\;=\;ventricular/intracranial\;volume\;\times\;10^3$$

PET imaging measures

The cerebral metabolic rate for glucose was determined by [18F] fludeoxyglucose (FDG) positron emission tomography (PET). A standard procedure for image pre-processing can be found at the following website: http://adni.loni.usc.edu/methods/pet-analysis/pre-processing/. Previous ADNI studies developed a “MetaROI” of the brain regions that demonstrate hypometabolic alterations among subjects with MCI and AD [46, 47]. These brain regions included bilateral posterior cingulate, right and left angular gyri, and right and left inferior temporal gyri. Global Standardized uptake value ratios (SUVRs) were calculated by averaging FDG uptake across the MetaROI and dividing by the pons and cerebellum.

Brain amyloid deposition was examined by [18F] florbetapir (AV45) PET as shown in http://www.adni-info.org. The mean AV45 uptake was determined within four regions, including frontal, lateral parietal, anterior/posterior cingulate, and lateral temporal regions. Global SUVRs were calculated by averaging across four brain regions and dividing by the whole cerebellum.

Genetic/CSF-based biomarkers

APOE (gene map locus 19Q13.2) genotypes of the study participants were extracted from the ADNI database. Participants with no APOE4 genotype were classified as APOE4 non-carriers while participants with at least one APOE4 genotype were categorized as APOE4 carriers. The levels of CSF Aβ42, total-tau (t-tau), and phosphorylated-tau at threonine 181 (p-tau) were determined by the Department of Pathology & Laboratory Medicine and Center for Neurodegenerative Disease Research, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania. The multiple xMAP Luminex platform and Innogenetics INNO-BIA AlzBio3 immunoassay reagents were used [48].

Clustering of longitudinal trajectories of cognitive performance

Given that ADAS-Cog is the gold standard for evaluating the efficacy of antidementia medications [49], ADAS-Cog-13 (a modified version of ADAS-Cog that includes more cognitive tasks than the original one and is thought to be more sensitive to cognitive impairment at the earliest stages of dementia) was used as our primary cognitive outcome. Only participants with at least one follow-up assessment of ADAS-Cog-13 were included in this analysis. To identify distinct longitudinal cognitive profiles, a non-parametric k-means longitudinal clustering method in the R package “kml” [50] was applied to detect trajectories of cognitive decline over a 5-year follow-up period. Before the clustering analysis, missing values were imputed by the “Copy Mean” method [50]. Briefly, the basic idea of this method is to impute missing values either using linear interpolation or last occurrence carried forward (LOCF) and then add a variation to make the individual trajectory similar to the “shape” of the overall sample’s average trajectory. We built the models for 1 to 8 clusters and selected the 4-cluster solution based on the Bayesian information criterion (BIC) [51] and the elbow method. A visual representation of the elbow method was created (see Additional file 1: Fig. S1). The raw individual trajectories and resultant 4-cluster trajectories are demonstrated in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Cognitive trajectories based on ADAS-Cog-13 from baseline to 5 years. Overall, 936 participants with MCI were included in this analysis, including those with at least 2 data points of ADAS-Cog-13 over a 5-year follow-up period. Four cognitive trajectories were identified by the longitudinal K-means cluster analysis. The solid blue, long-dash green, dash orange, and dot-dash red lines represent clusters 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively. The thin gray lines represent individual cognitive trajectories. Abbreviation: ADAS-Cog-13, Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive Subscale 13

Statistical analyses

At baseline, we used the R statistical software v4.1.2 [52] to explore the relationships between cluster membership and demographics, APOE4 status, neuropsychological evaluations, structural MRI assessments, PET imaging markers, and CSF AD pathologies. The differences between clusters were assessed with analysis of variance (ANOVA) for continuous variables and Pearson’s x 2 tests for categorical variables. When group differences were detected with ANOVA or Pearson’s x 2 tests, we used pairwise t-tests or x 2 tests in post hoc analyses and corrected for multiple testing using the false discovery rate (FDR) correction [53]. Group comparisons were also demonstrated visually as shown in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Baseline characteristics by cluster. Differences between clusters were assessed with ANOVA. When group differences were detected with ANOVA, we used pairwise t-tests in post hoc analyses and corrected for multiple testing using the FDR correction. Abbreviations: MCI, mild cognitive impairment; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; ADAS-Cog-13, Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive Subscale 13; aVV, adjusted ventricular volume; aEV, adjusted entorhinal cortex volume; aHV, adjusted hippocampal volume; FDG, fludeoxyglucose; SUVRs, standardized uptake value ratios; Aß42, ß-amyloid; t-tau, total tau; p-tau, phosphorylated tau

Linear mixed-effects models were used to examine the associations of cluster membership with the longitudinal change in cognition, structural MRI markers, PET imaging markers, and CSF AD biomarkers over up to 5 years from baseline. Eleven models were created for the following dependent variables: MMSE, memory composite score, executive function composite score, aHV, aEV, aVV, FDG SUVRs, AV45 SUVRs, CSF Aβ42, t-tau, and p-tau. Time since baseline (years), clusters, and their interaction were included as fixed effects. Age, gender, years of education, APOE4 status, and their interactions with time were included as covariates. All models included a random intercept for each participant. The model equations are as follows:

$$Y_{change}\;\sim\;Clusters^\ast time\;+\;Age^\ast time\;+\;Gender^\ast time\;+\;Education^\ast time\;+\;APOE4\;status^\ast time$$

where Y change is the change in each dependent variable from the baseline.

Additionally, to further understand the slope differences of these four cluster groups, pairwise comparisons between clusters using the estimated marginal means (EMMs) were performed and the FDR method was used to correct for multiple testing.

Kaplan-Meier curves were conducted to demonstrate the rate of conversion to dementia in the four clusters, and pairwise comparisons using log-rank tests were performed to compare survival curves. Follow-up duration was the number of years from baseline to dementia diagnosis at their last visit. During their follow-up period, subjects who did not convert to dementia were censored at their last visit.

Results

Findings of longitudinal k-means cluster analysis

As illustrated in Fig. 1, MCI subjects were assigned into the following clusters according to their cognitive trajectories: (1) cluster 1 with stable cognitive performance (we called this cluster “stable MCI,” n = 255, 27%), (2) cluster 2 with a mild cognitive decline (we called it “mild decliners,” n = 336, 35%), (3) cluster 3 with a moderate cognitive decline (we called it “moderate decliners,” n = 240, 26%), and (4) cluster 4 with a steep cognitive decline (we called it “aggressive decliners,” n = 105, 11%).

Baseline cluster characteristics

Table 1 shows the demographic and clinical characteristics by cluster. For age, the participants in the mild, moderate, and aggressive decliners groups were older than those in the stable MCI group, while no other pairwise difference was found. For education, the participants in the mild, moderate, and aggressive decliners groups were less educated than those in the stable MCI group, while no other pairwise difference was significant. For gender, the mild decliners group had a lower female proportion than the stable MCI group, while no other pairwise difference was observed. For APOE4 status, the participants in the moderate decliners and aggressive decliners groups had a higher proportion of APOE4 carriers than those in the stable MCI and mild decliners groups, while no other pairwise difference was found. For follow-up duration, the stable MCI group had a longer follow-up duration than all other groups, and the aggressive decliners group had a shorter follow-up duration than the mild decliners and moderate decliners groups. Regarding neuropsychological performance, all cognitive tests (MMSE, ADAS-Cog-13, memory composite score, and executive function composite score) showed significant differences between clusters. Regarding structural MRI assessments, four groups showed significant differences, with the exception of equivalent levels between the mild decliners and moderate decliners groups on aVV. For brain glucose metabolism, the moderate decliners and aggressive decliners groups did not differ in FDG SUVRs, while all other pairwise differences were significant. For both brain amyloid PET and CSF Aβ42, all pairwise differences were significant, with the exception of comparable levels between the moderate decliners and aggressive decliners groups. Regarding CSF tau pathologies, all pairwise differences were significant, with the exception of comparable levels between the moderate decliners and aggressive decliners groups on CSF p-tau. Figure 2 showing group differences is also created for visual inspection.

Table 1 Cluster characteristics at baseline

Cluster membership and longitudinal changes

The results from the linear mixed-effects models examining the associations between cluster membership and longitudinal changes in cognition, neurodegeneration, and CSF AD pathologies are displayed in Table 2 and Figs. 3 and 4.

Table 2 Summary of linear mixed-effects models
Fig. 3
figure 3

Forest plots showing the effect difference relative to stable MCI. Circles represent coefficients (as shown in Table 2), and horizontal dark lines represent the 95% confidence intervals. Abbreviations: MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; aVV, adjusted ventricular volume; aEV, adjusted entorhinal cortex volume; aHV, adjusted hippocampal volume; FDG, fludeoxyglucose; SUVRs, standardized uptake value ratios; Aß42, ß-amyloid; t-tau, total tau; p-tau, phosphorylated tau

Fig. 4
figure 4

Cluster membership and longitudinal changes in all major AD biomarkers over a 5-year follow-up. Intercepts and slopes of the four clusters come from linear mixed-effects models. There were significant differences between all four clusters in the amount of change in MMSE, memory, executive function, and aVV (A–D). All pairwise differences in the rates of decline in aEV, aHV, and FDG-PET were significant, with the exception of comparable levels between the cluster 3 and 4 groups (E–G). However, four clusters exhibited similar rates of change in Aβ-PET, CSF Aβ42, t-tau, and p-tau proteins (H–K). Abbreviations: MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; aVV, adjusted ventricular volume; aEV, adjusted entorhinal cortex volume; aHV, adjusted hippocampal volume; FDG, fludeoxyglucose; SUVRs, standardized uptake value ratios; Aß42, ß-amyloid; t-tau, total tau; p-tau, phosphorylated tau

For the models involving cognitive assessments (MMSE, the ADNI memory composite score, and the ADNI executive function composite score; see Table 2, Figs. 3A–C and 4A–C), the clusters (mild decliners, moderate decliners, and aggressive decliners) × time interactions were all significant such that the mild decliners, moderate decliners, and aggressive decliners groups had steeper slopes (i.e., faster cognitive decline) compared to the stable MCI group. To further understand the group differences in slope, post hoc analyses using the FDR correction were performed. The group differences in slope were significant in all pairwise comparisons (all FDR-adjusted p < 0.0001).

For the aVV model (see Table 2, Figs. 3D and 4D), the clusters × time interactions were all significant such that the mild decliners, moderate decliners, and aggressive decliners groups had steeper slopes on aVV (i.e., faster ventricular enlargement) compared to the stable MCI group. To further understand the group differences in slope, post hoc analyses using the FDR correction were performed. The group differences in slope were significant in all pairwise comparisons (all FDR-adjusted p < 0.0001).

For the aEV and aHV models (see Table 2, Figs. 3E, F and 4E, F), the clusters × time interactions were all significant such that the mild decliners, moderate decliners, and aggressive decliners groups had steeper slopes (i.e., faster entorhinal atrophy and faster hippocampal atrophy) compared to the stable MCI group. To further understand the group differences in slope, post hoc analyses using the FDR correction were performed. All pairwise differences in slope were significant, with the exception of comparable levels between the moderate decliners and aggressive decliners groups on entorhinal atrophy (coefficient: − 0.00551, SE: 0.01223, FDR-adjusted p: 0.6526) and on hippocampal atrophy (coefficient: 0.0117, SE: 0.0087, FDR-adjusted p: 0.1798).

For the FDG SUVRs model (see Table 2, Figs. 3G and 4G), the clusters × time interactions were all significant such that the mild decliners, moderate decliners, and aggressive decliners groups had steeper slopes on FDG SUVRs (i.e., faster decline in brain glucose metabolism) compared to the stable MCI group. To further understand the group differences in slope, post hoc analyses using the FDR correction were performed. All pairwise differences in slope were significant, with the exception of comparable levels between the moderate decliners and aggressive decliners groups on FDG SUVRs (coefficient: 0.0020, SE: 0.0030, FDR-adjusted p: 0.4963).

For the AV45 SUVRs model (see Table 2, Figs. 3H and 4H), the aggressive decliners × time term, but not the mild decliners × time or moderate decliners × time term, was significant, indicating that the aggressive decliners group rather than the mild decliners or moderate decliners group differed in slopes on AV45 SUVRs compared to the stable MCI group. When corrected by the FDR method, however, post hoc analyses did not find any pairwise differences in slopes (all FDR-adjusted p > 0.05).

For the CSF Aß42 model (see Table 2, Figs. 3I and 4I), the clusters × time interactions were not significant, suggesting that the mild decliners, moderate decliners, and aggressive decliners groups did not differ in slopes on CSF Aß42 compared to the stable MCI group. Furthermore, post hoc analyses also did not find any pairwise differences in slopes (all FDR-adjusted p > 0.05).

For the CSF t-tau model (see Table 2, Figs. 3J and 4J), the aggressive decliners × time term, but not the mild decliners × time or moderate decliners × time term, was significant, indicating that the aggressive decliners group rather than the mild decliners or moderate decliners group had a steeper slope on CSF t-tau compared to the stable MCI group. When corrected by the FDR method, however, post hoc analyses did not find any pairwise differences in slopes (all FDR-adjusted p > 0.05).

For the CSF p-tau model (see Table 2, Figs. 3K and 4K), the clusters × time interactions were not significant, suggesting that the mild decliners, moderate decliners, and aggressive decliners groups did not differ in slopes on CSF p-tau compared to the stable MCI group. Furthermore, post hoc analyses also did not find any pairwise differences in slopes (all FDR-adjusted p > 0.05).

Progression to dementia

Of the 936 subjects, 307 (32.8%) progressed to dementia within a 5-year follow-up period. Kaplan-Meier curves showing the rate of conversion to dementia are demonstrated in Fig. 5. A log-rank test found significant cluster differences in survival curves (x 2[3] = 462; p < 0.001). All pairwise comparisons with the FDR correction were significant (FDR-adjusted p < 0.001). Regarding the type of dementia, 294 of the 307 (95.8%) who converted to dementia were diagnosed with AD dementia. Thirteen subjects (4.2%) progressed to a non-AD dementia (4 frontal temporal dementia, 3 primary progressive aphasia, 1 progressive supranuclear palsy, 1 vascular dementia, 1 Shy-Drager syndrome, 1 semantic dementia, 1 with Parkinson’s disease and Lewy body dementia features, 1 other CNS disorder).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Kaplan-Meier survival curves showing the rate of progression to dementia in the four clusters. All clusters differed significantly from one another. Abbreviation: MCI, mild cognitive impairment

Sensitivity analyses

The residual approach was used to obtain adjusted MRI volumes to examine whether our MRI results are robust to a different ICV adjustment approach [54]. Coefficients and the 95% confidence intervals of the three linear mixed-effects models were summarized (see Additional file 1: Fig. S2). Compared to the results of Fig. 3.D–F, the patterns of coefficients and the 95% confidence intervals of these three models remained unchanged.

Discussion

There were four key findings of the current study. First, there is substantial heterogeneity in disease progression among MCI subjects despite being intentionally recruited as an independent clinical entity often thought of as the prodromal stage of dementia [6]. Second, we identified a considerable portion of subjects with MCI (cluster 1) showing a very little cognitive decline over 5 years of follow-up. This group of individuals exhibited a remarkably benign-looking biomarker profile. Third, individuals in the cluster 2 and 3 groups demonstrated relatively mild and moderate cognitive decline trajectories, respectively. Fourth, a subgroup of individuals with MCI (cluster 4) exhibited an aggressive cognitive decline trajectory and was characterized by a pronouncedly abnormal biomarker profile.

We found substantial differences between the four clusters in clinical characteristics and longitudinal changes in major AD biomarkers, indicating that our data-driven clustering method categorized MCI subjects into biologically and clinically different subgroups. Cluster 1, comprising about 27% of our study sample, showed a nearly non-existent rate of change in the ADAS-Cog-13 over a 5-year follow-up period. This subgroup had remarkably better baseline performance on the ADAS-Cog-13, MMSE, memory composite, and executive function scores; a more healthy-looking biomarker profile; and a substantially slower change in clinical progression relative to the rest of the MCI group. In agreement with this finding, previously published studies conducted in a population-based sample [30] and the ADNI data set [55] also identified a very similar MCI subgroup, which had less impaired baseline performance on cognitive tasks and a substantially lower risk of clinical progression compared to other MCI groups. This subgroup is especially relevant in clinical trials because the inclusion of “disease-free” MCI subjects may minimize the potential to detect the beneficial effects of treatment for MCI [12]. Edmonds and colleagues suggest that the identification and removal of this subgroup could maximize the capability to observe the treatment effects of new therapeutics in clinical trials involving subjects with MCI [12]. Better stratification of MCI populations before recruitment in clinical trials may help increase the chances of observing efficacy and will contribute to the development of more efficient study designs.

Cluster 2, the largest MCI subgroup identified in this study (n = 336, 35%), initially performed worse (indicated by a larger intercept in Fig. 1) on ADAS-Cog-13 and exhibited slightly faster cognitive deterioration over time relative to cluster 1. Moreover, levels of AD-associated biomarkers differed significantly between the cluster 1 and 2 subgroups (Fig. 2). Namely, at baseline, participants in cluster 2 had higher levels of CSF tau pathologies and lower levels of CSF Aβ42 compared to those in cluster 1. These findings are in accordance with earlier longitudinal studies where they have shown that abnormal CSF biomarker profiles are predictive of conversion from MCI to AD with high accuracy [56, 57]. We also found more impaired cognitive performance, as evaluated with the MMSE, memory, and executive function composite scores, in the cluster 2 group, compared to the cluster 1 group (Fig. 2). These two subgroups demonstrated pronounced differences in Aβ-PET SUVRs, a biomarker of amyloid accumulation in the brain, and FDG-PET SUVRs, a marker of neurodegeneration and synaptic dysfunction [58]. Our findings are consistent with a previous PET imaging study, which suggests that Aβ deposition is a robust predictor of clinical progression from MCI to AD [59], with amyloid changes occurring long before the start of cognitive decline [60]. Likewise, the finding that cluster 1 had higher levels of FDG-PET SUVRs (representing less severe neurodegeneration) relative to cluster 2 agrees with the observation that higher levels of FDG-PET SUVRs are associated with remaining cognitively stable among individuals with MCI [61].

Clusters 3 (n = 240, 26%) and 4 (n = 105, 11%) exhibited substantially steeper cognitive deterioration over time compared to clusters 1 and 2 (Fig. 1), with participants in the cluster 4 group showing the most aggressive cognitive decline trajectory. These findings may have a critical impact on potentiating clinical trials involving MCI subjects based on the predicted magnitude of change in cognition (e.g., ADAS-Cog-13) over time. For example, it is likely that future trials may attempt to enroll those subjects who would be predicted to fall into the cluster 4 group since the inclusion of these aggressive cognitive decliners may enhance the probability of success in clinical trials and lead to a significant gain of power to observe treatment effects. However, this approach should be conducted with caution because cluster 4 is a relatively small group (n = 105, 11%). The inclusion of only those subjects in cluster 4 would likely hinder clinical trials, as participants in other subgroups (89%) would be excluded. Furthermore, future studies should focus on the development of statistical models predicting cluster membership using baseline demographics and clinical characteristics to facilitate the recruitment process for clinical trials.

The amyloid cascade hypothesis of AD postulates that the pathologic process initiates with amyloid deposition (as measured by CSF Aβ42 and Aβ-PET), followed by changes in CSF tau proteins, then changes in FDG-PET and structural MRI, followed by cognitive symptoms [44, 62]. Our results largely support this conceptual model. Specifically, our linear mixed-effects models with four cognitive trajectories as the independent variable and all major AD biomarkers as dependent variables (Fig. 4) found that four distinct cognitive decline trajectories (i.e., different rates of cognitive decline) had comparable rates of changes in Aβ-PET, CSF Aβ42, and tau proteins (Fig. 4H–K) but exhibited significantly different rates of changes in structural MRI and FDG-PET (Fig. 4D–G) at the MCI stage of dementia, in accordance with the notion that cognitive decline is only loosely coupled with changes in Aβ-PET and CSF Aβ42 [63, 64], but is tightly accompanied by changes in markers of neurodegeneration [65,66,67] at the later stages of the disease (e.g., MCI and AD dementia). For instance, previous studies found that among patients who were experiencing a rapid cognitive decline (e.g., AD patients), rates of MRI changes were correlated with cognitive deterioration, while rates of brain amyloid deposition were not [64, 68]. In addition, Vemuri and colleagues found that among subjects with MCI, correlations with cognitive measures were strong with MRI volumes but were not significant with levels of CSF tau [69]. As expected, participants with four distinct cognitive decline trajectories identified by the cluster technique based on longitudinal ADAS-Cog-13 data also exhibited different rates of other cognitive outcomes (i.e., MMSE, memory, and executive function; Fig. 4A–C), and all pairwise differences in rates of cognitive decline were significant. Somewhat unexpectedly, we observed a significant difference between cluster 3 and cluster 4 in the rate of change in aVV (i.e., widening ventricles; Fig. 4D), but not aEV, aHV, or FDG-PET (Fig. 4E–G), despite that all of these imaging markers are thought of as neurodegenerative markers. This discrepancy may be attributed to the fact that relative to medial temporal atrophy (i.e., aEV and aHV) or hypometabolism on FDG-PET, the enlargement of ventricles is considered to be a more downstream event and more strongly coupled with a change in global cognition over time [70]. It is also likely that several factors that we did not examine in the present study, such as cognitive reserve, brain resilience, and other brain neuropathologies (e.g., vascular damages, Lewy bodies), may contribute to the difference in the rate of cognitive decline between clusters 3 and 4 [71, 72].

This study has several limitations. First, we observed highly variable individual trajectories on the ADAS-Cog-13 among individuals with MCI (i.e., the thin gray lines in Fig. 1). In this study, the usage of cluster analysis should be interpreted as an exploratory analysis in nature, rather than a confirmative one. We acknowledge that a larger sample size of each subgroup, particularly the cluster 4 group, would be warranted to yield more robust and generalizable findings. However, our linear mixed-effects models with cluster membership as the independent variable and other cognitive outcomes (i.e., MMSE, memory, and executive function) produced a very consistent pattern of cognitive trajectories (Fig. 4A–C), further supporting the notion that the four trajectories identified in the cluster analysis were stable and robust. Second, we did not use or incorporate other AD biological biomarkers in the clustering process, since our primary study goal was to examine the heterogeneity in cognitive decline, and the ADAS-Cog-13 is the most predominant assessment used to track disease progression in AD clinical trials [49]. Third, changes in AD biomarkers over long periods are non-linear [62] but were modeled as linear in our linear mixed-effects models. Nevertheless, over a shorter period, changes in AD biomarkers can be modeled as linear functions since such non-linearity seems to be minimal [73]. Fourth, the ADNI memory composite score was derived from several memory assessments, such as memory tasks of ADAS-Cog. This may introduce some degree of circularity since the ADNI memory composite score partly overlaps with the ADAS-Cog-13.

In conclusion, we identified four distinct cognitive decline trajectories of MCI and further characterized changes in all major AD biomarkers over time for each subgroup. Our findings highlight the importance of considering the heterogeneity of MCI when recruiting participants in clinical trials, thus potentially contributing to better trial design and more precise personalized medicine.

Availability of data and materials

Data used in the present study has been made publicly available by the ADNI in the Laboratory of Neuro Imaging (LONI) database.

References

  1. Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, Geda YE, Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, Jack CR Jr. Mild cognitive impairment: ten years later. Arch Neurol. 2009;66(12):1447–55.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Manly JJ, Tang MX, Schupf N, Stern Y, Vonsattel JP, Mayeux R. Frequency and course of mild cognitive impairment in a multiethnic community. Ann Neurol. 2008;63(4):494–506.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. DeCarli C. Mild cognitive impairment: prevalence, prognosis, aetiology, and treatment. Lancet Neurol. 2003;2(1):15–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Nettiksimmons J, DeCarli C, Landau S, Beckett L. Biological heterogeneity in ADNI amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(5):511-521.e511.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Bowen JB, Teri L, Kukull WA, McCormick WC, McCurry SM, Larson EB. Progression to dementia in patients with isolated memory loss. The Lancet. 1997;349:763–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56(3):303–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Petersen RC, Stevens JC, Ganguli M, Tangalos EG, Cummings J, DeKosky ST. Practice parameter: early detection of dementia: mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review). Neurology. 2001;56:1133–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Pandya S, Clem MA, Silva LM, Woon FLM. Does mild cognitive impairment always lead to dementia? a review. J Neurol Sci. 2016;369:57–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Mielke MM, Cha R, Pankratz VS, Christianson TJH, Geda YE, Boeve BF, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, et al. Higher risk of progression to dementia in mild cognitive impairment cases who revert to normal. Neurology. 2014;82:317–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Mehta D, Jackson RL, Paul G, Shi J, Sabbagh MS. Why do trials for Alzheimer’s disease drugs keep failing? a discontinued drug perspective for 2010–2015. Expert Opin Investig Drugs. 2017;26:735–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Jutten RJ, Sikkes SAM, van der Flier WM, Scheltens P, Visser PJ, Tijms BM. Finding treatment effects in Alzheimer trials in the face of disease progression heterogeneity. Neurology. 2021;96:e2673–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Edmonds EC, Ard MC, Edland SD, Galasko DR, Salmon DP, Bondi MW. Unmasking the benefits of donepezil via psychometrically precise identification of mild cognitive impairment: a secondary analysis of the ADCS vitamin E and donepezil in MCI study. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;4:11–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Kwak K, Giovanello KS, Bozoki AC, Styner M, Dayan E. Subtyping of mild cognitive impairment using a deep learning model based on brain atrophy patterns. Cell Rep Med. 2021;2(12):100467.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Gamberger D, Lavrač N, Srivatsa S, Tanzi RE, Doraiswamy PM. Identification of clusters of rapid and slow decliners among subjects at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):6763.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Nezhadmoghadam F, Martinez-Torteya A, Treviño V, Martínez E, Santos A, Tamez-Peña J. Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging I: robust discovery of mild cognitive impairment subtypes and their risk of Alzheimer’s disease conversion using unsupervised machine learning and gaussian mixture modeling. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2021;18(7):595–606.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Katabathula S, Davis PB, Xu R. Comorbidity-driven multi-modal subtype analysis in mild cognitive impairment of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2022.

  17. Hamilton CA, Matthews FE, Donaghy PC, Taylor JP, O’Brien JT, Barnett N, Olsen K, McKeith IG, Thomas AJ. Prospective predictors of decline v. stability in mild cognitive impairment with Lewy bodies or Alzheimer’s disease. Psychol Med  2021;51(15):2590–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Blanken AE, Jang JY, Ho JK, Edmonds EC, Han SD, Bangen KJ, Nation DA. Distilling heterogeneity of mild cognitive impairment in the National Alzheimer coordinating center database using latent profile analysis. JAMA Network Open. 2020;3(3):200413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Hanfelt JJ, Peng L, Goldstein FC, Lah JJ. Latent classes of mild cognitive impairment are associated with clinical outcomes and neuropathology: analysis of data from the National Alzheimer’s coordinating center. Neurobiol Dis. 2018;117:62–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Delano-Wood L, Bondi MW, Sacco JM, Abeles N, Jak AJ, Libon DJ, Bozoki AC. Heterogeneity in mild cognitive impairment: differences in neuropsychological profile and associated white matter lesion pathology. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15:906–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Köhler S, Hamel R, Sistermans N, Koene T, Pijnenburg YA, van der Flier WM, Scheltens P, Visser PJ, Aalten P, Verhey FR, et al. Progression to dementia in memory clinic patients without dementia: a latent profile analysis. Neurology. 2013;81(15):1342–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Libon DJ, Xie SX, Eppig JS, Wicas G, Lamar M, Lippa CF, Bettcher BM, Price CC, Giovannetti T, Swenson R, et al. The heterogeneity of mild cognitive impairment: a neuropsychological analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;16:84–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Edmonds EC, Delano-Wood L, Jak AJ, Galasko DR, Salmon DP, Bondi MW. “Missed” mild cognitive impairment: high false-negative error rate based on conventional diagnostic criteria. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;52(2):685–91.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Bondi MW, Edmonds EC, Jak AJ, Clark LR, Delano-Wood L, McDonald CR, Nation DA, Libon DJ, Au R, Galasko D, et al. Neuropsychological criteria for mild cognitive impairment improves diagnostic precision, biomarker associations, and progression rates. J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;42(1):275–89.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Eppig JS, Edmonds EC, Campbell L, Sanderson-Cimino M, Delano-Wood L, Bondi MW. Statistically derived subtypes and associations with cerebrospinal fluid and genetic biomarkers in mild cognitive impairment: a latent profile analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2017;23(7):564–76.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Thomas KR, Bangen KJ, Weigand AJ, Ortiz G, Walker KS, Salmon DP, Bondi MW, Edmonds EC. Cognitive heterogeneity and risk of progression in data-driven subtle cognitive decline phenotypes. J Alzheimers Dis. 2022;90:323–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Giraldo DL, Sijbers J, Romero E. Quantification of cognitive impairment to characterize heterogeneity of patients at risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2021;13(1): e12237.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Edmonds EC, Smirnov DS, Thomas KR, Graves LV, Bangen KJ, Delano-Wood L, Galasko DR, Salmon DP, Bondi MW. Data-driven vs consensus diagnosis of MCI: enhanced sensitivity for detection of clinical, biomarker, and neuropathologic outcomes. Neurology. 2021;97(13):e1288–99.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Clark L, Delano-Wood L, Libon DJ, McDonald CR, Nation DA, Bangen KJ, Jak AJ, Au R, Salmon DP, Bondi MW. Are empirically-derived subtypes of mild cognitive impairment consistent with conventional subtypes? J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013;19:635–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Machulda MM, Lundt ES, Albertson SM, Kremers WK, Mielke MM, Knopman DS, Bondi MW, Petersen RC. Neuropsychological subtypes of incident mild cognitive impairment in the mayo clinic study of aging. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15(7):878–87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Xie H, Mayo NE, Koski L. Identifying and characterizing trajectories of cognitive change in older persons with mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2011;31:165–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Lee JS, Cho S-K, Kim HJ, Kim YJ, Park K-C, Lockhart SN, Na DL, Kim C, Seo SW. Prediction models of cognitive trajectories in patients with nonamnestic mild cognitive impairment. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):10468.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Kim YJ, Cho SK, Kim HJ, Lee JS, Lee J, Jang YK, Vogel JW, Na DL, Kim C, Seo SW. Data-driven prognostic features of cognitive trajectories in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairments. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2019;11(1):10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Kim BS, Jun S, Kim H: Cognitive trajectories and associated biomarkers in patients with mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis 2023.

  35. Raghavan N, Samtani MN, Farnum M, Yang E, Novak G, Grundman M, Narayan VA, Dibernardo AB. The ADAS-Cog revisited: novel composite scales based on ADAS-Cog to improve efficiency in MCI and early AD trials. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9:S21–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Kim SJ. Woo S-y, Kim YJ, Jang H, Kim HJ, Na DL, Kim S, Seo SW, Initiative tAsDN: development of prediction models for distinguishable cognitive trajectories in patients with amyloid positive mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2022;114:84–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43(11):2412–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL. A new rating scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry. 1984;141(11):1356–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Schmidt M: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test A Handbook. 1996.

  41. Crane PK, Carle AC, Gibbons LE, Insel PS, Mackin RS, Gross AL, Jones RN, Mukherjee S, Curtis SM, Harvey DJ, et al. Development and assessment of a composite score for memory in the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). Brain Imaging Behav. 2012;6:502–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Gibbons LE, Carle AC, Mackin RS, Harvey DJ, Mukherjee S, Insel PS, Curtis SM, Mungas D, Crane PK. Initiative FtAsDN: A composite score for executive functioning, validated in Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) participants with baseline mild cognitive impairment. Brain Imaging Behav. 2012;6:517–27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Jack CR, Jr., Bernstein MA, Fox NC, Thompson P, Alexander G, Harvey D, Borowski B, Britson PJ, J LW, Ward C et al: The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI): MRI methods. J Magn Reson Imaging 2008, 27(4):685-691.

  44. Jack CR Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Shaw LM, Aisen PS, Weiner MW, Petersen RC, Trojanowski JQ. Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer’s pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9(1):119–28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Nestor SM, Rupsingh R, Borrie M, Smith M, Accomazzi V, Wells JL, Fogarty J, Bartha R. Ventricular enlargement as a possible measure of Alzheimer’s disease progression validated using the Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative database. Brain. 2008;131(Pt 9):2443–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Landau SM, Harvey D, Madison CM, Koeppe RA, Reiman EM, Foster NL, Weiner MW, Jagust WJ. Associations between cognitive, functional, and FDG-PET measures of decline in AD and MCI. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32(7):1207–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Jagust WJ, Bandy D, Chen K, Foster NL, Landau SM, Mathis CA, Price JC, Reiman EM, Skovronsky D, Koeppe RA. The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative positron emission tomography core. Alzheimers Dement. 2010;6(3):221–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Shaw LM, Vanderstichele H, Knapik-Czajka M, Clark CM, Aisen PS, Petersen RC, Blennow K, Soares H, Simon A, Lewczuk P, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid biomarker signature in Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative subjects. Ann Neurol. 2009;65(4):403–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Kueper JK, Speechley M, Montero-Odasso M. The Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive Subscale (ADAS-Cog): modifications and responsiveness in pre-dementia populations. a narrative review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;63(2):423–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Genolini C, Alacoque X, Sentenac M, Arnaud C. kml and kml3d: R packages to cluster longitudinal data. J Stat Softw. 2015;065:1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Schwarz G. Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann Stat. 1978;6:461–4.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Team RC: R: a language and environment for statistical computing. MSOR connections 2014, 1.

  53. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc B: Stat Methodol. 1995;57:289–300.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Voevodskaya O, Simmons A, Nordenskjöld R, Kullberg J, Ahlström H, Lind L, Wahlund LO, Larsson EM, Westman E. The effects of intracranial volume adjustment approaches on multiple regional MRI volumes in healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Front Aging Neurosci. 2014;6:264.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Edmonds EC, Delano-Wood L, Clark L, Jak AJ, Nation DA, McDonald CR, Libon DJ, Au R, Galasko D, Salmon DP, et al. Susceptibility of the conventional criteria for mild cognitive impairment to false-positive diagnostic errors. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11:415–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Hansson O, Zetterberg H, Buchhave P, Londos E, Blennow K, Minthon L. Association between CSF biomarkers and incipient Alzheimer’s disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a follow-up study. The Lancet Neurology. 2006;5:228–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Mattsson N, Zetterberg H, Hansson O, Andreasen N, Parnetti L, Jonsson MAG, Herukka SK, van der Flier WM, Blankenstein MA, Ewers M, et al. CSF biomarkers and incipient Alzheimer disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. JAMA. 2009;302(4):385–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Jack CR, Bennett DA, Blennow K, Carrillo MC, Dunn B, Haeberlein SLB, Holtzman DM, Jagust WJ, Jessen F, Karlawish J, et al. NIA-AA Research Framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14:535–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Pascoal TA, Therriault J, Mathotaarachchi S, Kang MS, Shin M, Benedet AL, Chamoun M, Tissot C, Lussier FZ, Mohaddes S, et al. Topographical distribution of Aβ predicts progression to dementia in Aβ positive mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2020;12(1):12037.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Jansen WJ, Ossenkoppele R, Knol DL, Tijms BM, Scheltens P, Verhey FRJ, Visser PJ, Aalten P, Aarsland D, Alcolea D, et al. Prevalence of cerebral amyloid pathology in persons without dementia: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2015;313(19):1924–38.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Iaccarino L, Sala A, Perani D. Predicting long-term clinical stability in amyloid-positive subjects by FDG-PET. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2019;6:1113–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Jack CR, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Petersen RC, Weiner MW, Aisen PS, Shaw LM, Vemuri P, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, et al. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. The Lancet Neurology. 2013;12:207–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Jack CR, Lowe VJ, Senjem ML, Weigand SD, Kemp BJ, Shiung MM, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, Klunk WE, Mathis CA, et al. 11C PiB and structural MRI provide complementary information in imaging of Alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Brain. 2008;131(Pt 3):665–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Jack CR, Lowe VJ, Weigand SD, Wiste HJ, Senjem ML, Knopman DS, Shiung MM, Gunter JL, Boeve BF, Kemp BJ, et al. Serial PIB and MRI in normal, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: implications for sequence of pathological events in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2009;132:1355–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Hyman BT. Amyloid-dependent and amyloid-independent stages of Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2011;68(8):1062–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Fox NC, Scahill RI, Crum WR, Rossor MN. Correlation between rates of brain atrophy and cognitive decline in AD. Neurology. 1999;52:1687–1687.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Jack CR, Petersen RC, Xu YC, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Boeve BF, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Rates of hippocampal atrophy correlate with change in clinical status in aging and AD. Neurology. 2000;55:484–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Engler H, Forsberg A, Almkvist O, Blomquist G, Larsson E, Savitcheva I, Wall A, Ringheim A, Långström B, Nordberg A. Two-year follow-up of amyloid deposition in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2006;129(Pt 11):2856–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Vemuri P, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Shaw LM, Trojanowski JQ, Weiner MW, Knopman DS, Petersen RC, Jack CR Jr. MRI and CSF biomarkers in normal, MCI, and AD subjects: diagnostic discrimination and cognitive correlations. Neurology. 2009;73(4):287–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Jack CR, Shiung MM, Gunter JL, O’brien PC, Weigand SD, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, Cha R, et al. Comparison of different MRI brain atrophy rate measures with clinical disease progression in AD. Neurology  2004;62:591–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2006;20(3 Suppl 2):S69-74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Nelson PT, Abner EL, Schmitt FA, Kryscio RJ, Jicha GA, Smith CD, Davis DG, Poduska JW, Patel E, Mendiondo MS, et al. Modeling the association between 43 different clinical and pathological variables and the severity of cognitive impairment in a large autopsy cohort of elderly persons. Brain Pathol. 2010;20(1):66–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Luo JR, Agboola F, Grant EA, Morris JC, Masters CL, Albert MS, Johnson SC, McDade E, Fagan AM, Benzinger TL-S, et al. Accelerated longitudinal changes and ordering of Alzheimer disease biomarkers across the adult lifespan. Brain. 2022;145(12):4459–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Data collection and sharing for this project was funded by the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) (National Institutes of Health Grant U01 AG024904) and DOD ADNI (Department of Defense award number W81XWH-12-2-0012). ADNI is funded by the National Institute on Aging, the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering, and through generous contributions from the following: AbbVie, Alzheimer’s Association; Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery Foundation; Araclon Biotech; BioClinica, Inc.; Biogen; Bristol-Myers Squibb Company; CereSpir, Inc.; Cogstate; Eisai Inc.; Elan Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; Eli Lilly and Company; EuroImmun; F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd. and its affiliated company Genentech, Inc.; Fujirebio; GE Healthcare; IXICO Ltd.; Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy Research & Development, LLC.; Johnson & Johnson Pharmaceutical Research & Development LLC.; Lumosity; Lundbeck; Merck & Co., Inc.; Meso Scale Diagnostics, LLC.; NeuroRx Research; Neurotrack Technologies; Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation; Pfizer Inc.; Piramal Imaging; Servier; Takeda Pharmaceutical Company; and Transition Therapeutics. The Canadian Institutes of Health Research is providing funds to support ADNI clinical sites in Canada. Private sector contributions are facilitated by the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health (www.fnih.org). The grantee organization is the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, and the study is coordinated by the Alzheimer’s Therapeutic Research Institute at the University of Southern California. ADNI data are disseminated by the Laboratory for Neuro Imaging at the University of Southern California.

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database (adni.loni.usc.edu). As such, the investigators within the ADNI contributed to the design and implementation of ADNI and/or provided data but did not participate in the analysis or writing of this report. A complete listing of ADNI investigators can be found at http://adni.loni.usc.edu/wp-content/uploads/how_to_apply/ADNI_Acknowledgement_List.pdf.

Funding

This study did not receive any grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Contributions

XWW, TY, and WJZ drafted the manuscript. WJZ and JZ supervised the study. WJZ and JZ performed the clustering analyses and statistical work. WJZ and JZ contributed to the data management and revision of the manuscript. All authors discussed the results and commented on the paper. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Wenjun Zhou or Jie Zhang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Each ADNI participant or authorized representative provided written informed consent, and the institutional review board of each participating ADNI site approved the ADNI study. This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. This project was also submitted for review to the institutional review board of Wenzhou Seventh People’s Hospital. However, given that this study did not involve contact with human subjects and used de-identified data, the institutional review board of Wenzhou Seventh People’s Hospital determined this study did not require review.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Jie Zhang serves as the Founder of Hangzhou Shansier Medical Technologies Co., Ltd., and holds shares in Hangzhou Shansier Medical Technologies Co., Ltd., which is dedicated to digitizing clinical practice and research. Wenjun Zhou is employed by Hangzhou Shansier Medical Technologies Co., Ltd., and holds shares in Hangzhou Shansier Medical Technologies Co., Ltd. The other authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wang, X., Ye, T., Zhou, W. et al. Uncovering heterogeneous cognitive trajectories in mild cognitive impairment: a data-driven approach. Alz Res Therapy 15, 57 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-023-01205-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-023-01205-w

Keywords