Skip to main content

Cognitively normal women with Alzheimer’s disease proteinopathy show relative preservation of memory but not of hippocampal volume

Abstract

Background

We examined interactive effects of sex, diagnosis, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) amyloid beta/phosphorylated tau ratio (Aβ/P-tau) on verbal memory and hippocampal volumes.

Methods

We assessed 682 participants (350 women) from BioFINDER (250 cognitively normal [CN]; and 432 symptomatic: 186 subjective cognitive decline [SCD], 246 mild cognitive impairment [MCI]). General linear models evaluated effects of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) proteinopathy (CSF Aß/p-tau ratio), diagnosis, and sex on verbal memory (ADAS-cog 10-word recall), semantic fluency (animal naming fluency), visuospatial skills (cube copy), processing speed/attention functions (Symbol Digit Modalities Test and Trail Making Part A), and hippocampal volumes.

Results

Amyloid-positive (Aβ/P-tau+) CN women (women with preclinical AD) showed memory equivalent to amyloid-negative (Aβ/P-tau−) CN women. In contrast, Aβ/P-tau+ CN men (men with preclinical AD) showed poorer memory than Aβ/P-tau− CN men. Symptomatic groups showed no sex differences in effect of AD proteinopathy on memory. There was no interactive effect of sex, diagnosis, and Aβ/P-tau on other measures of cognition or on hippocampal volume.

Conclusions

CN women show relatively preserved verbal memory, but not general cognitive reserve or preserved hippocampal volume in the presence of Aβ/P-tau+. Results have implications for diagnosing AD in women, and for clinical trials.

Background

Memory differs between women and men in ways that may meaningfully impact the detection and course of Alzheimer’s disease (AD). In particular, women have verbal memory strengths that appear to be sustained early in the disease, despite measurable pathological changes, including fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography (FDG PET) abnormalities [1], hippocampal volume (HV) loss [2], and amyloid beta (Aβ) protein accumulation as shown by a positive amyloid PET scan [3, 4]. This early preserved memory may delay diagnosis [5], which is troubling given evidence that women—and particularly those with risk factors such as Apolipoprotein ε4 (APOE ε4) genotype—may decline faster than men, once AD-related cognitive decline has begun [6,7,8,9]. The exact timing of women’s memory changes in AD remains unknown and a topic of interest [10].

Memory advantages in women throughout the lifespan could relate to a variety of etiological factors (e.g., [9, 11,12,13,14]). Proximally, preservation of memory is expected to be reflected in resilience in neural structures and functions that subserve healthy memory. Although memory relies on a complex network of neural regions interacting effectively, the central role of the hippocampus in episodic memory (for a review, see [15]) and in AD [16] makes this structure a natural candidate for underlying neural resilience. Specifically, the hippocampus is critical for memory consolidation, and damage to the hippocampus can lead to decreased ability to learn and recall new information [15]. In AD, the hippocampus is impacted by pathology early in the disease course, with neurofibrillary tangle buildup beginning in the cornu ammonis 1 (CA1) and subiculum regions and progressing throughout the hippocampus in a predictable fashion [16].

Previous work from our group has built preliminary support for relatively preserved total and right hippocampal volume (HV) [3] and right subiculum subfield volume [17] in women. Specifically, with a trend-level finding, our data suggested that sex moderated the effects of diagnosis and amyloid PET positivity on HV. In women, positive amyloid PET related to smaller HV only at the mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stage, but not in cognitively normal women. The pattern in men was similar, but the differences were not significant [3]. When we examined these findings by hippocampal subfield, we replicated the effect only within the right subiculum [17].

Importantly, if sex relates to a time-limited preservation of memory and/or HV in AD, analyses of sex effects should consider both diagnostic trajectory (i.e., normal cognition to MCI to dementia) and presence of biomarkers such as brain amyloid and tau measured by PET or indexed by the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). This process allows for higher certainty that sex effects are occurring in AD and not general to non-AD cognitive impairment.

Work on sex differences in AD has been done with large, well-characterized study samples, and especially in one study population (the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative [ADNI]). However, a key step in understanding sex differences in AD is exploring whether findings within ADNI replicate in different samples and with different measures.

The current investigation sought to examine whether the effects of AD proteinopathy on memory and hippocampal volume differ based on sex in the Swedish BioFINDER cohort. BioFINDER offers advantages in that it includes a clinically representative sample of patients who were consecutively referred to participating memory clinics and control participants recruited from an ongoing population-based study (the Malmö Diet and Cancer study) [18]. Both groups on average have levels of education more typical of an aging population than in some other large study samples [19] and were scanned on the same magnetic resonance (MR) scanner. BioFINDER also includes measures of other cognitive domains, including visuospatial skills, attention, and processing speed skills, allowing for assessment of the specificity of effects to the memory domain. Altogether, these advantages position the BioFINDER [20] sample well to identify associations between biomarkers of pathological change and cognition.

Based on our prior results [3, 17], we hypothesized that women would show early relative preservation of memory and HV, such that these variables would be impacted less by AD proteinopathy (Aβ/P-tau+) in CN women than in CN men. In other words, women, but not men, with preclinical AD would show early preservation of these variables. We hypothesized this effect would be specific to memory and not generalized to other cognitive domains. We further hypothesized that at the symptomatic stage, Aβ+ women would no longer show relative preservation of memory and HV, and no sex differences would be observed.

Methods

Participants

Participants were selected from the Swedish BioFINDER study, which is a prospective, longitudinal study examining disease mechanisms in AD and other neurodegenerative disorders using several fluid and imaging biomarkers (see http://biofinder.se for more information about the study design). For the present study, we included participants with CSF and MRI data from the healthy elderly control cohort (n = 250) and all non-demented patients that had been referred to participating memory clinics due to cognitive symptoms (n = 432).

The CN elderly participants were consecutively enrolled from a population-based cohort in the South of Sweden (Malmö Diet and Cancer Study [18]). The inclusion criteria were (1) age ≥ 60 years old, (2) Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [21] score of 28–30 points, and (3) fluent in Swedish. The exclusion criteria were (1) presence of subjective cognitive impairment, (2) significant neurologic disease (for example, stroke, Parkinson’s disease, multiple sclerosis), (3) severe psychiatric disease (for example, severe depression or psychotic syndromes), and (4) dementia or mild cognitive impairment (MCI).

The patients with cognitive symptoms (“symptomatic patients”) had all been referred to one of three participating memory clinics in the south of Sweden, mostly from primary care, and consecutively enrolled in BioFINDER based on the following inclusion and exclusion criteria: (1) perceived cognitive decline, (2) did not fulfill Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-5) [22] criteria for Major Neurocognitive Disorder (dementia), as assessed by a memory clinic physician, (3) had a MMSE score of 24 to 30 points, (4) were aged 60 to 80 years, and (5) were fluent in Swedish. The exclusion criteria were (1) cognitive impairment that could be explained by another condition (other than prodromal dementia), such as brain tumor, (2) severe somatic disease, and (3) refusing lumbar puncture or neuropsychological testing.

The patients were further categorized as having subjective cognitive decline (SCD) (n = 186) or MCI (n = 246). The MCI classification was based on the results of a comprehensive neuropsychological battery and the clinical assessment of a senior neuropsychologist and two physicians [23]. Patients with composite z-scores of ≤ − 1.5 standard deviations (SD) in at least one cognitive domain were classified as MCI (at least two different tests were used for each cognitive domain). In agreement with the DSM-5 criteria for mild neurocognitive disorders, all subjects with z-scores of − 1 to − 1.5 were individually assessed by the neuropsychologist and classified as MCI if their premorbid ability or individual test scores within each domain indicated a significant cognitive decline. Among the MCI participants, 75% were categorized as amnestic MCI and 25% as non-amnestic MCI. The participants with cognitive complaints who did not fulfill the criteria for MCI or dementia were classified as having SCD. Neuropsychological test measures incorporated in the following statistical analyses were not included in the battery used to determine diagnosis.

The study was approved by the ethical review board in Lund, Sweden, and all participants gave their written informed consent.

CSF analysis and classification of Aβ/P-tau status

Lumbar puncture (LP) and CSF procedures followed a previously described protocol [22]. CSF Aβ42 and phosphorylated tau (P-tau) were analyzed using the Elecsys immunoassays on a cobas e601 analyzer at the Clinical Neurochemistry Laboratory, University of Gothenburg, Sweden.

Aβ/P-tau positivity was defined based on a previously defined CSF cut-off (phosphorylated tau/Aβ42 ratio ≥ 0.022). This cutoff has been validated against FDA-approved visual reads of Aβ PET scans with 90% agreement [24].

MRI procedures and hippocampal volume

All participants were examined using the same MR scanner (3 Tesla Siemens Tim Trio). The MR scanning and imaging procedures have been described previously [25]. FreeSurfer software (version 5.3) was used to extract data for the total intracranial volume and total HV (left and right).

Memory function

The 10-word list from the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale – cognitive subscale (ADAS-cog) was used to test memory function [26]. After three learning trials of 10 words (immediate recall) and a distraction task (naming objects and fingers), the participants were asked to recall the 10 words. The number of omissions on the delayed recall task (i.e., total possible recalled—total recalled) constituted the final test score. Commission errors were not analyzed.

Non-memory functions

Semantic fluency was assessed using the animal naming fluency test [27]. Visuospatial function was assessed with a cube copying task [28] scored from 0 to 6 points [26]. Attention and speed were assessed using Trail Making Test Part A (TMT A) [29] and the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) [30].

Statistical analysis

Groups were compared using the Mann-Whitney Test. A p value of < 0.05 was used to define statistical significance. Several general linear models (GLMs) were used to test the effect of sex on cognitive performance in the presence of AD proteinopathy at different diagnostic stages. First, we examined memory function (delayed recall omissions) as the dependent variable with the independent variables Aβ/P-tau status, diagnostic group, sex, years of formal education, presence of at least one APOE ε4 allele, HV, and total intracranial volume. Interaction effects were tested for Aβ/P-tau status, diagnostic group, and sex. This model was also run using animal naming fluency, cube copying, TMT A, and SDMT as dependent variables. Diagnostic group was primarily stratified into CN and symptomatic patients to achieve better statistical power for the primary analyses; in secondary analyses, the symptomatic patients were further stratified into SCD and MCI patients. To further examine sex differences in the effect of AD proteinopathy on memory, we ran GLMs using delayed recall omissions as dependent variable and Aβ/P-tau, age, and education as independent variables. This model was tested separately for men and women in all diagnostic subgroups (primary analyses separately in CN and symptomatic patients, and in secondary analyses with symptomatic patients stratified into SCD and MCI). Here, (Aβ/P-tau+) CN women are referred to as women with preclinical AD, and (Aβ/P-tau+) CN men are similarly referred to as men with preclinical AD.

Next, sex differences in the effect of AD proteinopathy on HV were examined in the different diagnostic groups. Here, we used models with HV as a dependent variable with the independent variables Aβ/P-tau, sex, diagnostic group, age, education, presence of at least one APOE ε4 allele, and total intracranial volume. Total HV, left HV, and right HV were used separately as dependent variables. As for the GLMs described above, interaction effects were tested for Aβ/P-tau status, diagnostic group, and sex (as above, diagnostic group was stratified both as CN or symptomatic patient and, in secondary analyses as CN, SCD, or MCI). In the secondary analyses where symptomatic patients were further stratified into SCD and MCI, interaction effects were analyzed with Mann-Whitney tests due to smaller sample sizes and skewed distributions. All statistical analysis was performed using R version 3.4.4 (The R Foundation for Statistical Computing).

Results

Demographics

Of 682 participants, 350 were women, 241 were Aβ/P-tau+, and 267 were APOE ε4 carriers. Regarding diagnosis, 250 were CN and 432 had cognitive symptoms (of which 186 had subjective and 246 objective symptoms). Average age was 71.7 (SD = 5.5). Of CN participants, 152 were women, 67 were APOE ε4 carriers (39 women), and 45 were Aβ/P-tau+ (29 women). Of symptomatic patients, 198 were women, 200 were APOE ε4carriers (90 women), and 196 were Aβ/P-tau+ (88 women). See Tables 1 and 2 for additional demographic and descriptive information.

Table 1 Means and standard deviations by diagnostic group (cognitively normal or symptomatic), sex, and Aβ/P-tau status for demographics; memory and global cognitive scores, hippocampal and total intracranial volumes; and number of APOE ε4 carriers
Table 2 Means and standard deviations by symptomatic diagnostic group (subjective cognitive decline or mild cognitive impairment), sex, and Aβ/P-tau status for demographics; memory and global cognitive scores, hippocampal and total intracranial volumes; and number of APOE ε4 carriers

Among CN participants, Mann-Whitney tests showed that age and education did not differ by amyloid status (age: W = 3955.5, p = 0.13; education: W = 4433.0, p = 0.68), but a greater percentage of Aβ/P-tau+ versus Aβ/P-tau− CN individuals were APOE ε4 carriers (W = 2482.0, p < 0.001). Mann-Whitney tests also revealed CN women were older (W = 6439.5, p = 0.05), but CN men and women did not differ in education level (W = 7969.0, p = 0.35) or number of APOE ε4 carriers (W = 7490.0, p = 0.57) (see Table 1).

Within symptomatic participants, Mann-Whitney tests showed that Aβ/P-tau+ individuals were older (W = 17,639.0, p < 0.001) and were more likely to be APOE ε4 carriers (W = 12,647.0, p < 0.001). Symptomatic Aβ/P-tau+ versus Aβ/P-tau− individuals did not differ in education level (W = 23,862.0, p = 0.36). Mann-Whitney tests also revealed men and women with cognitive symptoms did not differ in education level (W = 21,386.0, p = 0.30) or number of APOE ε4 carriers (W = 23,418.0, p = 0.69), but men were older (W = 25,686.0, p = 0.05) (see Table 1).

Secondary Mann-Whitney analyses examining differences within SCD and MCI individuals showed that within SCD, Aβ/P-tau+ individuals were older (W = 2852.5, p < 0.01), more likely to be APOE ε4 carriers (W = 2345.0, p < 0.001), and had lower education levels (W = 4576.5, p = 0.015). Mann-Whitney tests revealed men and women with SCD did not differ in education level (W = 414,105.0, p = 0.68) or number of APOE ε4 carriers (W = 4382.00, p = 0.53), but men were older (W = 5094.5, p = 0.03). For MCI individuals, Aβ/P-tau+ individuals were older (W = 5778.0, p < 0.01), more likely to be APOE ε4 carriers (W = 3743.0, p < 0.001), and had higher education levels (W = 6019.5, p = 0.03). Mann-Whitney tests showed men and women with SCD did not differ in education level (W = 6814.0, p = 0.83), number of APOE ε4 carriers (W = 6990.0, p = 0.62), or age (W = 7455.5, p = 0.67) (see Table 2).

Interactive effects of sex, diagnosis, and amyloid status on delayed verbal recall

The GLM with sex, diagnosis (CN or symptomatic), Aβ/P-tau+, and their interactions predicting delayed recall omissions on the ADAS word recall task showed a significant three-way interaction of Aβ/P-tau+, diagnosis, and sex (p < 0.001) as well as a two-way interaction of Aβ/P-tau+ and sex (p = 0.008). These interactions were observed in a model including age, education, APOE ε4 status, and total HV as covariates (Table 3). A secondary analysis that grouped diagnoses as CN, SCD, or MCI showed a similar three-way interaction of Aβ/P-tau+, diagnosis, and sex (p = 0.01; see Table 4).

Table 3 Results for regression models with sex, diagnosis (cognitively normal or symptomatic), Aβ/P-tau status, and their interactions predicting recall omissions on the ADAS word recall task and total hippocampal volume
Table 4 Results for secondary regression model with sex, diagnosis (cognitively normal, subjective cognitive decline, or mild cognitive impairment), Aβ/Ptau status, and their interactions predicting recall omissions on the ADAS word recall task

Parsing this interaction effect indicated that women but not men showed a significant interaction between amyloid status and diagnosis (subsample with men, p = 0.42; subsample with women, p < 0.001). Specifically, Aβ/P-tau+ did not impact delayed recall memory in women in the CN group (p = 0.45, adjusted for age and education), but related to poorer memory in symptomatic women (p < 0.001). In contrast, Aβ/P-tau+ was associated with poorer memory performance both in CN men (p = 0.02) and in symptomatic men (p = 0.003) adjusted for age and education (Fig. 1). A secondary analysis, splitting the symptomatic group into SCD and MCI, showed Aβ/P-tau+ related to poorer memory both in female SCD (p = 0.02) and MCI (p < 0.001) and in male SCD (p = 0.057) and MCI (p = 0.006) (see Additional file 1: Figure S1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Comparison of the Aβ/P-tau effect on memory in men and women. Comparison of 10 word delayed recall performance in Aβ/P-tau+ and Aβ/P-tau− participants, stratified by sex, in a healthy controls and b patients with cognitive symptoms. Comparisons were adjusted for age and education. Further comparison among the symptomatic patients (SCD and MCI) is shown in Additional file 1: Figure S1

Interactive effects of sex, diagnosis, and amyloid status on verbal semantic fluency, visuospatial function, and attention/processing speed

The above models with sex, diagnosis (CN or patients with cognitive symptoms), Aβ/P-tau+, and their interactions were also tested for predicting performance on verbal semantic fluency (animal naming fluency), visuospatial function (cube copying), and attention/processing speed (TMT A and SDMT). The models were, as for memory function, adjusted for age, education, APOE ε4 status, and total HV. In contrast to using memory performance as outcome, the three-way interaction of Aβ/P-tau+, diagnosis, and sex was not significant for verbal fluency (p = 0.11), visuospatial function (p = 0.60), or attention/processing speed (TMT A, p = 0.86; SDMT, p = 0.96).

Interactive effects of sex, diagnosis, and amyloid status on hippocampal volume

In contrast to the memory analysis, the GLM with sex, diagnosis, Aβ/P-tau+, and their interactions predicting total HV did not show a significant interaction of Aβ/P-tau+, diagnosis, and sex (p = 0.91). However, diagnosis, age, and total intracranial volume (ICV) showed significant main effects on HV. Specifically, older age was associated with smaller HV (p < 0.001), as was having cognitive symptoms (p < 0.001). Greater total intracranial volume was associated with greater HV (p < 0.001) (see Table 3 for details). Similar significant main effects and similar lack of interaction effects were seen when grouping diagnoses into CN, SCD, and MCI and when left and right HV were examined separately (interaction: left: p = 0.68; right: p = 0.85; total p = 0.91) (see Additional file 2 for details).

Discussion

The current study showed that women with AD proteinopathy (Aβ/P-tau+) showed no memory impairment relative to Aβ/P-tau− women prior to self-reporting concerns about their cognition (i.e., only in the CN group). In contrast, men with Aβ/P-tau+ performed more poorly on the verbal memory task than Aβ/P-tau− men, regardless of whether they experienced cognitive symptoms or not. As hypothesized, similar effects were not seen on tests of other cognitive domains. Analyses examining HV showed no significant interactive effects of sex, diagnosis, and Aβ/P-tau+, and diagnosis alone of these three factors showed a significant main effect on HV.

These results supported our hypotheses about sex-based preservation of verbal memory. Namely, Aβ/P-tau+ CN women (i.e., women with preclinical AD) appear to have verbal memory reserve or resilience in the presence of measurable AD-related disease burden. This result is consistent with our and others’ work showing that women’s memory has some early resilience to a number of markers of AD burden, including abnormal brain metabolism [1], hippocampal atrophy [2], and positive amyloid PET [3, 4]. Importantly, the current results replicate our prior findings in a separate sample that incorporated different measures of memory and amyloid proteinopathy, supporting the robustness of this finding [3]. Early verbal memory preservation may have implications for how normal cognition is defined in research and in clinical trials. For example, it may be more critical when examining longitudinal or interventional outcomes to define baseline group membership using biomarkers in combination with cognitive testing, if women are included in the sample. From a clinical diagnosis perspective, this finding emphasizes the importance of cognitive baseline assessment, as early neuropsychological testing may be able to identify women who have normal cognition yet have lost some memory functioning over time. Employing memory assessments that are not purely verbal may also be important for increasing validity of memory assessment across sexes.

Supplementary analyses showed that verbal memory was not preserved in Aβ/P-tau+ women with SCD as compared to Aβ/P-tau− women with SCD. In addition, in the SCD group, women no longer outperformed men in verbal memory. This pattern suggests that among women with increased risk for AD dementia (Aβ/P-tau+), memory reserve or resilience is limited to CN women with no cognitive concerns. This finding is consistent with studies showing that SCD is associated with positive AD biomarkers and longitudinal cognitive decline ([for recent review on SCD, see [31]), as well as with research showing that AD proteinopathy in the context of SCD is a strong predictor of decline [32]. However, this finding stands in contrast to research within the ADNI sample, showing that women outperform men on verbal memory tasks despite mild to moderate levels of AD-related burden [1, 2]. It is possible that the current finding relates to Aβ/P-tau+ women with SCD in the BioFINDER sample having more than a moderate level of AD-related disease burden. Alternatively, the finding may relate to other differences between samples. Specifically, the greater education levels—and thereby greater general cognitive reserve—in the ADNI sample may have an impact. On the other hand, in the BioFINDER sample, individuals with SCD have been referred to a memory specialist due to cognitive symptoms, whereas in ADNI, individuals with SCD reported symptoms when queried. In this context, our findings suggest that for women seeking a memory specialist, SCD could be not only a marker of risk, but also an indicator that subclinical memory changes are already measurable on cognitive testing. This suggestion is in part consistent with very recent work in other cohorts [33, 34].

The current study did not confirm hypotheses about sex-specific HV preservation. Previously, we have shown in the ADNI sample that CN women with positive amyloid PET studies show no difference in total HV and subiculum subfield volume, compared to CN women with negative amyloid PET, but that volumetric decrements are observed in amyloid-positive women at the MCI stage [3, 17]. Reasons for lack of replication are unclear but could include slight differences in methodology or sample composition, such as inclusion of CN and symptomatic groups in the present analysis as well as differences in the biomarkers used to define the presence of proteinopathy. Lack of replication emphasizes the need for additional research on how this structure—with known developmental [35], aging-related [36, 37], and neurochemical sex differences [38]—does or does not show patterns of dysfunction and atrophy that differ by sex in AD. In addition, this finding underscores the importance of considering other neural underpinnings of early sex-based memory preservation and later decline.

As important context, there are known sex by APOE ε4 interactive effects, with evidence for more deleterious effects of APOE ε4 on cognition, hippocampal structure, brain function at rest, and tau pathology in women than men ([6, 39], for a recent review see [40]). The present analysis showed that men and women did not differ in number of APOE ε4 carriers and also controlled for effects of APOE ε4. Despite these efforts to show our findings were not driven by APOE ε4 status, adding APOE ε4 as an interaction term was beyond the scope of the present analysis due to limited power. Further work is needed on how memory reserve presents over the AD spectrum in women with APOE ε4.

Limitations of this analysis include having a smaller sample size than studies that combine across cohorts, which is particularly relevant when evaluating complex interactions. Although the current study in part replicates prior work by our group [3], the challenges of complex interactions and multiple comparisons mean that wider replication is important to ensure generalizable conclusions. Expanding the analysis to additional regions of interest will also be important for generalizability. The sample included is also majority white, and research in diverse samples will be key to generalizing the findings to all women. The current analysis was also cross-sectional in nature, limiting ability to interpret memory findings as true losses of function over time. In contrast, the present study has strengths in that participants are more representative of the general aging population than in some other cohorts [18, 19], were more thoroughly assessed and diagnosed, and have had brain imaging conducted on the same MRI magnet.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this study shows relative preservation of verbal memory in the presence of AD proteinopathy, limited to women with normal cognition, and not in women with reported or measured memory symptoms. This resilience was specific to memory and was not present for other cognitive functions. Future studies should examine other potential neural sources of sex-based early memory preservation, conduct additional multi-cohort analyses of complex sex-based interactive effects—including longitudinally—and examine the practical effects of sex differences in memory on clinical diagnosis and clinical trial inclusion and outcomes.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from authors Sebastian Palmqvist and Oskar Hansson on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

AD:

Alzheimer’s disease

ADAS-Cog:

Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale – cognition

ADNI:

Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative

Aβ:

Amyloid beta

CN:

Cognitively normal

CSF:

Cerebrospinal fluid

FDG PET:

Fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography

GLM:

General linear models

ICV:

Total intracranial volume

LP:

Lumbar puncture

MCI:

Mild cognitive impairment

MMSE:

Mini-Mental State Examination

MR:

Magnetic resonance

P-tau:

Phosphorylated Tau

SCD:

Subjective cognitive decline

References

  1. Sundermann EE, Maki PM, Rubin LH, Lipton RB, Landau S, Biegon A. Female advantage in verbal memory evidence of sex-specific cognitive reserve. Neurology. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000003288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Sundermann EE, Biegon A, Rubin LH, Lipton RB, Mowrey W, Landau S, et al. Better verbal memory in women than men in MCI despite similar levels of hippocampal atrophy. Neurology. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000002570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Caldwell JZK, Berg J-L, Cummings JL, Banks SJ. Moderating effects of sex on the impact of diagnosis and amyloid positivity on verbal memory and hippocampal volume. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-017-0300-8.

  4. Sundermann EE, Biegon A, Rubin LH, Lipton RB, Landau S, Maki PM. Does the female advantage in verbal memory contribute to underestimating Alzheimer’s disease pathology in women versus men? J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;56:947–57.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Brunet H, Caldwell JZK, Brandt J, Miller JB. Influence of sex differences in interpreting learning and memory within a clinical sample of older adults. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2019.1566433.

  6. Buckley RF, Mormino EC, Amariglio RE, Properzi MJ, Rabin JS, Lim YY, et al. Sex, amyloid, and APOE ε4 and risk of cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: findings from three well-characterized cohorts. Alzheimers Dement. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.04.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Sohn D, Shpanskaya K, Lucas JE, Petrella JR, Saykin AJ, Tanzi RE, et al. Sex differences in cognitive decline in subjects with high likelihood of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease. Sci Rep. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25377-w.

  8. Burke SL, Hu T, Fava NM, Li T, Rodriguez MJ, Schuldiner KL, et al. Sex differences in the development of mild cognitive impairment and probable Alzheimer's disease as predicted by hippocampal volume or white matter hyperintensities. J Women Aging. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1080/08952841.2018.1419476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Goodman Y, Bruce AJ, Cheng B, Mattson MP. Estrogens attenuate and corticosterone exacerbates excitotoxicity, oxidative injury, and amyloid-peptide toxicity in hippocampal neurons. J Neurochem. 1996;66:1836–44.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Nebel RA, Aggarwal NT, Barnes LL, Gallagher A, Goldstein JM, Kantarci K, et al. Understanding the impact of sex and gender in Alzheimer’s disease: a call to action. Alzheimers Dement. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.04.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Pike CJ. Estrogen modulates neuronal Bcl-x(L) expression and-amyloid-induced apoptosis: relevance to Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurochem. 1999. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1471-4159.1999.721552.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Neu SC, Pa J, Kukull W, Beekly D, Kuzma A, Gangadharan P, et al. Apolipoprotein E genotype and sex risk factors for Alzheimer disease: a meta-analysis. JAMA Neurol. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.2188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Mangold CA, Wronowski B, Du M, Masser DR, Hadad N, Bixler GV, et al. Sexually divergent induction of microglial-associated neuroinflammation with hippocampal aging. J Neuroinflammation. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12974-017-0920-8.

  14. Zhao L, Mao Z, Woody SK, Brinton RD. Sex differences in metabolic aging of the brain: insights into female susceptibility to Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.02.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Wixted JT, Cai DJ. Memory consolidation. Oxford Handb Cogn Neurosci. 2013:1–59. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199988693.013.0021.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  16. Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological staging of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 1991. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00308809.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Caldwell JZK, Berg JL, Shan G, Cummings JL, Banks SJ. Sex moderates the impact of diagnosis and amyloid PET positivity on hippocampal subfield volume. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-180028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Borland E, Nägga K, Nilsson PM, Minthon L, Nilsson ED, Palmqvist S. The Montreal cognitive assessment: normative data from a large Swedish population-based cohort. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-170203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Huisman M, Kunst AE, Mackenbach JP. Socioeconomic inequalities in morbidity among the elderly: a European overview. Soc Sci Med. 2003. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00454-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. The Swedish BIOFINDER Study. Available from: http://biofinder.se. Accessed 19 Oct 2018.

  21. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

  23. Palmqvist S, Zetterberg H, Blennow K, Vestberg S, Andreasson U, Brooks DJ, et al. Accuracy of brain amyloid detection in clinical practice using cerebrospinal fluid β-amyloid 42: a cross-validation study against amyloid positron emission tomography. JAMA Neurol. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Hansson O, Seibyl J, Stomrud E, Zetterberg H, Trojanowski JQ, Bittner T, et al. CSF biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease concord with amyloid-β PET and predict clinical progression: a study of fully automated immunoassays in BioFINDER and ADNI cohorts. Alzheimers Dement. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.01.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Palmqvist S, Schöll M, Strandberg O, Mattsson N, Stomrud E, Zetterberg H, et al. Earliest accumulation of β-amyloid occurs within the default-mode network and concurrently affects brain connectivity. Nat Commun. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-01150-x.

  26. Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL. A new rating scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry. 1984. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.141.11.1356.

  27. Tallberg IM, Ivachova E, Jones Tinghag K, Ostberg P. Swedish norms for word fluency tests: FAS, animals and verbs. Scand J Psychol. 2008. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00653.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Palmqvist S, Hansson O, Minthon L, Londos E. The usefulness of cube copying for evaluating treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Dement. 2008. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317508320084.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Reitan RM. The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J Consult Psychol. 1955. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0044509.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Fleisher AS, Sowell BB, Taylor C, Peterson RC, Thal LJ, et al. Clinical predictors of progression to Alzheimer’s disease in amnestic mild cognitive impairment; 2007. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000258542.58725.4c.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  31. Jessen F, Amariglio RE, Van Boxtel M, Breteler M, Ceccaldi M, Chételat G, et al. A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative (SCD-I) working group. Alzheimers Dement. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Van Harten AC, Visser PJ, Pijnenburg YAL, Teunissen CE, Blankenstein MA, Scheltens P, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid Aβ42 is the best predictor of clinical progression in patients with subjective complaints. Alzheimers Dement. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2012.08.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Sundermann E, Edmonds EC, Delano-Wood L, Galasko DR, Salmon DP, Rubin LH, et al. Sex influences the accuracy of subjective memory complaint reporting in older adults. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-170425.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Buckley RF, Villemagne VL, Masters CL, Ellis KA, Rowe CC, Johnson KA, et al. A conceptualization of the utility of subjective cognitive decline in clinical trials of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. J Mol Neurosci. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000868.Child-evoked.

  35. Krogsrud SK, Tamnes CK, Fjell AM, Amlien I, Grydeland H, Sulutvedt U, et al. Development of hippocampal subfield volumes from 4 to 22 years. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Ardekani BA, Convit A, Bachman AH. Analysis of the MIRIAD data shows sex differences in hippocampal atrophy progression. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-150780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Nosheny RL, Insel PS, Truran D, Schuff N, Jack CR, Aisen PS, et al. Variables associated with hippocampal atrophy rate in normal aging and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.07.036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Marrocco J, McEwen BS. Sex in the brain: Hormones and sex differences. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2016. https://doi.org/10.12032/TMR201705034.

  39. Buckley RF, Mormino EC, Chhatwal J, Schultz AP, Rabin JS, Rentz DM, et al.; for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Associations between baseline amyloid, sex, and APOE on subsequent tau accumulation in cerebrospinal fluid. Neurobiol Aging. 2019; doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.02.019.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Riedel BC, Thompson PM, Brinton RD. Age, APOE and sex: triad of risk of Alzheimer’s disease. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsbmb.2016.03.012.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank MacKenzie Leavitt for his assistance with manuscript preparation.

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health, The Women’s Alzheimer’s Movement, a Center for Biomedical Research Excellence award from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences (P20GM109025), the European Research Council, the Swedish Research Council, the Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation, the Marianne and Marcus Wallenberg Foundation, the Strategic Research Area MultiPark (Multidisciplinary Research in Parkinson’s disease) at Lund University, the Swedish Alzheimer Association, the Swedish Brain Foundation, The Parkinson foundation of Sweden, The Parkinson Research Foundation, the Skåne University Hospital Foundation, and the Swedish federal government under the ALF agreement.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JZKC conceptualized the study and wrote the manuscript. JLC provided expertise and critical feedback into manuscript development. SJB provided critical input to the study design and manuscript development. SP analyzed data, provided critical input to analysis, and wrote the manuscript. OH provided critical input to analysis and manuscript development. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jessica Z. K. Caldwell.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Regional Ethics Committee in Lund, Sweden, approved the study design. All patients gave their written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

OH has acquired research support (for the institution) from Roche, GE Healthcare, Biogen, AVID Radiopharmaceuticals, Fujirebio, and Euroimmun. In the past 2 years, he has received consultancy/speaker fees (paid to the institution) from Lilly, Roche, and Fujirebio.

JZKC and SJB declare that they have no competing interests.

JLC has provided consultation to Acadia, Accera, Actinogen, Alkahest, Allergan, Alzheon, Avanir, Axsome, BiOasis Technologies, Biogen, Diadem, EIP Pharma, Eisai, Genentech, Green Valley, Grifols, Hisun, Idorsia, Kyowa Kirin, Lilly, Lundbeck, Merck, Otsuka, Proclara, QR, Resverlogix, Roche, Samus, Samumed, Sunovion, Suven, Takeda, Teva, Toyama, and United Neuroscience pharmaceutical and assessment companies. JLC acknowledges funding from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences (Grant: P20GM109025) and support from Keep Memory Alive.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1:

Figure S1. Comparison of the Aβ/P-tau effect on memory in men and women. Comparison of 10 word delayed recall performance in Aβ/P-tau+ and Aβ/P-tau- participants, stratified by sex, in patients with A. subjective cognitive decline, and B. mild cognitive symptoms.

Additional file 2:

Regression Predicting Hippocampal Volumes with Cognitively Normal, Subjective Cognitive Decline, and Mild Cognitive Impairment Groups. This additional file contains a table summarizing the results of a regression predicting hippocampal volumes separated by diagnostic group.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Caldwell, J.Z.K., Cummings, J.L., Banks, S.J. et al. Cognitively normal women with Alzheimer’s disease proteinopathy show relative preservation of memory but not of hippocampal volume. Alz Res Therapy 11, 109 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-019-0565-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-019-0565-1

Keywords