Skip to main content

Combining tau-PET and fMRI meta-analyses for patient-centered prediction of cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease

Abstract

Background

Tau-PET is a prognostic marker for cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease, and the heterogeneity of tau-PET patterns matches cognitive symptom heterogeneity. Thus, tau-PET may allow precision-medicine prediction of individual tau-related cognitive trajectories, which can be important for determining patient-specific cognitive endpoints in clinical trials. Here, we aimed to examine whether tau-PET in cognitive-domain-specific brain regions, identified via fMRI meta-analyses, allows the prediction of domain-specific cognitive decline. Further, we aimed to determine whether tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composites capture patient-specific cognitive trajectories more sensitively than conventional cognitive measures.

Methods

We included Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) participants classified as controls (i.e., amyloid-negative, cognitively normal, n = 121) or Alzheimer’s disease-spectrum (i.e., amyloid-positive, cognitively normal to dementia, n = 140), plus 111 AVID-1451-A05 participants for independent validation (controls/Alzheimer’s disease-spectrum=46/65). All participants underwent baseline 18F-flortaucipir tau-PET, amyloid-PET, and longitudinal cognitive testing to assess annual cognitive changes (i.e., episodic memory, language, executive functioning, visuospatial). Cognitive changes were calculated using linear mixed models. Independent meta-analytical task-fMRI activation maps for each included cognitive domain were obtained from the Neurosynth database and applied to tau-PET to determine tau-PET signal in cognitive-domain-specific brain regions. In bootstrapped linear regression, we assessed the strength of the relationship (i.e., partial R2) between cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET vs. global or temporal-lobe tau-PET and cognitive changes. Further, we used tau-PET-based prediction of domain-specific decline to compose personalized cognitive composites that were tailored to capture patient-specific cognitive decline.

Results

In both amyloid-positive cohorts (ADNI [age = 75.99±7.69] and A05 [age = 74.03±9.03]), cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET outperformed global and temporal-lobe tau-PET for predicting future cognitive decline in episodic memory, language, executive functioning, and visuospatial abilities. Further, a tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composite across cognitive domains enhanced the sensitivity to assess cognitive decline in amyloid-positive subjects, yielding lower sample sizes required for detecting simulated intervention effects compared to conventional cognitive endpoints (i.e., memory composite, global cognitive composite). However, the latter effect was less strong in A05 compared to the ADNI cohort.

Conclusion

Combining tau-PET with task-fMRI-derived maps of major cognitive domains facilitates the prediction of domain-specific cognitive decline. This approach may help to increase the sensitivity to detect Alzheimer’s disease-related cognitive decline and to determine personalized cognitive endpoints in clinical trials.

Background

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is characterized by the accumulation of beta-amyloid (Aβ) and tau pathology ensuing neurodegeneration and cognitive decline [1, 2]. While neuroimaging and fluid biomarkers of Aβ and tau are routinely established for AD diagnosis [2, 3], reliable prognosis of cognitive decline remains a critical challenge, which is further complicated by the considerable heterogeneity in symptom manifestation and disease progression [4,5,6].

Accurate prediction of cognitive decline in AD will be, however, particularly important for clinical trials to (i) stratify patients by progression risk and/or (ii) determine patient-specific clinical endpoints that allow sensitive assessment of heterogeneous cognitive trajectories within trial-typical 1–2-year follow-ups [4, 7, 8]. Elevated tau-PET is closely associated with the development of cognitive deficits in AD, and recent studies also support tau-PET as a promising prognostic marker for cognitive decline, outperforming the prognostic accuracy of amyloid-PET and MRI-assessed neurodegeneration [9,10,11]. Specifically, elevated tau-PET has been associated with faster global cognitive decline and clinical conversion to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD dementia [9, 10, 12, 13]. Moreover, cross-sectional studies reported that tau-PET patterns closely mirror clinical AD heterogeneity, including occipital tau-PET in posterior cortical atrophy (i.e., visual-variant AD), left-hemispheric tau-PET in language-variant AD, and mesial-temporal-lobe tau in amnestic AD [14, 15]. This suggests a strong clinico-pathological correspondence between symptom manifestation and tau-PET patterns. Thus, mapping tau-PET to brain networks associated with cognitive domains typically affected in AD may allow the prediction of cognitive-domain-specific decline, to facilitate the assessment of patient-specific cognitive trajectories in clinical trials targeting tau pathology [16].

To test this, we obtained data from two independent cohorts from the Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative (ADNI, N = 261) and the A05 cohort (N = 111) covering healthy controls (i.e., cognitively normal Aβ−) and AD-spectrum patients (i.e., Aβ+) with baseline flortaucipir tau-PET and amyloid-PET. Further, all participants were characterized on different cognitive domains including episodic memory (MEM), language (LAN), executive functioning (EF), and visuospatial abilities (VS) across ~1.5–2 years of follow-up, which matches follow-up periods of several recent phase 2b&3 clinical trials (e.g., EMERGE/ENGAGE) [8]. To assess the association between tau-PET and cognitive-domain-specific decline, we obtained meta-analytical maps of task-fMRI studies from Neurosynth [17, 18] to determine brain regions that are consistently associated with MEM/LAN/EF/VS across ~1900 task-fMRI studies. We then mapped patient-specific tau-PET to cognitive-domain-specific brain activation maps and tested whether determining baseline tau-PET in regions involved in a given cognitive domain improves the prediction of cognitive-domain-specific decline compared to conventional tau-PET metrics (i.e., global/temporal-lobe tau-PET standardized uptake value ratios [SUVRs]) that have been suggested as prognostic markers in AD [9, 10]. Lastly, we used tau-PET predicted domain-specific decline to compute patient-specific weighted cognitive composites, which we hypothesized to capture individual tau-related cognitive trajectories more sensitively than commonly used cognitive outcomes. Using simulated interventions, we then tested whether tau-PET-informed cognitive composites (i.e., personalized endpoints) [19] improve the sensitivity to detect treatment effects compared to conventional cognitive scores.

Materials and methods

Participants

ADNI

We included 261 ADNI participants (inclusion criteria: https://adni.loni.usc.edu/wp-content/uploads/2010/09/ADNI_GeneralProceduresManual.pdf) based on the availability of 18F-florbetapir/18F-florbetaben amyloid-PET, 18F-flortaucipir tau-PET, longitudinal cognitive assessments (>1 follow-up), demographics (age, sex, education), clinical status, and APOE genotyping. All baseline data were obtained within 6 months; clinical status was classified by ADNI investigators as cognitively normal (CN, Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE] ≥ 24, Clinical Dementia Rating [CDR] = 0, non-depressed), MCI (MMSE ≥ 24, CDR = 0.5, objective memory-impairment on education-adjusted Wechsler Memory Scale II, preserved activities of daily living), or demented (MMSE = 20–26, CDR ≥ 0.5, NINCDS/ADRDA criteria for probable AD). Subjects with non-AD-related cognitive impairment (i.e., Aβ− MCI/dementia) were excluded. Subjects were classified as APOE4 risk allele carriers when at least one ε4 allele was detected. Ethics approval was obtained by ADNI investigators; all study participants provided written informed consent.

A05

A total of 111 participants were selected from the AVID-1451-A05 phase 2/3 trial (NCT02016560; inclusion criteria: https://clinicaltrials.gov/ProvidedDocs/60/NCT02016560/Prot_000.pdf), based on the availability of 18F-florbetapir amyloid-PET, 18F-flortaucipir tau-PET, longitudinal cognitive assessments (≥ 1follow-up), demographics (age, sex), clinical status, and APOE genotyping. Continuous measures on years of education were not available for all participants and thus not included. All baseline data were obtained within 30 days. Participants were classified as CN (MMSE ≥ 29, no history of cognitive impairment), MCI (24 ≤ MMSE < 29, showing MCI according to NIA-AA working group’s diagnostic guidelines) [20], and AD dementia (10 < MMSE < 24, showing possible or probable AD based on NIA-AA working group’s diagnostic guidelines) [21]. As for ADNI, subjects with non-AD-related cognitive impairment (i.e., Aβ− MCI/dementia) were excluded. Subjects were classified as APOE4 risk allele carriers when at least one ε4 allele was detected. The study was approved by the clinical investigator’s local Institutional Review Board; all participants provided written informed consent.

fMRI meta-analyses of cognitive-domain-specific brain activation and tau-PET assessments

To determine tau-PET in regions that are critical for a given cognitive domain (i.e., MEM/LAN/EF/VS), we obtained meta-analytical brain activation maps from task-based fMRI studies from Neurosynth (https://neurosynth.org/analyses/terms/, see Additional file 1: Methods) using the search terms: episodic memory (included studies, N = 332), language (included studies, N = 1101), executive control (included studies, N = 230), and visuospatial (included studies, N = 267). Methodological details on Neurosynth-based meta-analyses have been described previously (see https://neurosynth.org/faq/) [17]. Cognitive-domain-specific meta-analytical task-fMRI maps were binarized and masked with cortical gray matter to exclude typical tau-PET off-target binding regions (i.e., hippocampus/subcortex) [22, 23] and applied to spatially normalized SUVR-transformed tau-PET images to extract tau-PET signal within cognitive-domain-specific brain regions (for details on PET acquisition and preprocessing see Additional file 1: Methods). To further minimize influences of flortaucipir off-target binding, we used voxel-wise two-component Gaussian-mixture-modeling to transform SUVRs to tau-positivity probabilities (TPP) [24, 25]. Repeating this procedure for each task-fMRI map yielded cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET signals for MEM/LAN/EF/VS (Fig. 1A, B (I)). To compare the accuracy of cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET for predicting cognitive trajectories against conventional tau-PET measures, we obtained global and temporal-lobe tau-PET SUVRs, which have been previously shown to capture AD-related tau accumulation and predict cognitive decline [9, 10, 26, 27]. For global tau-PET, we determined average neocortical tau-PET, while excluding typical off-target binding regions (hippocampus, subcortex, cerebellum [22, 23]; Fig. 1B (II)). The temporal-lobe ROI included Braak-stage ROIs 1, 3, and 4 (Fig. 1B (II)) [24, 28].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Generation of meta-analytical task-fMRI maps for assessing cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET (A), surface rendering of regions of interests (ROIs) applied to tau-PET (B), and analysis flowchart (C). First, we compared the predictive accuracy of global tau-PET, temporal tau-PET, and cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET for predicting future cognitive decline using bootstrapped linear regressions. Note that separate regression models were run for each tau-PET ROI (CI). Second, we ran 1000 bootstrapped linear regression models in ADNI Aβ+ and extracted beta values for each variable of the regression model (CII), which were later used for assessing patient-specific cognitive composites in both ADNI Aβ+ and A05 Aβ+ (CIII). Specifically, we applied the 1000 bootstrapped linear model equations to subject level data to determine predicted cognitive change rates for each Aβ+ subject of the ADNI and A05 sample. Based on the rank of the predicted cognitive change rates, we then determined a tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composite. Abbreviations: DV, dependent variable; IV, independent variable; CV, control variable; CR, change rate

Assessment of cognitive changes

For both samples, we obtained composites of MEM/LAN/EF/VS to assess cognitive-domain-specific changes. In ADNI, we used pre-established composites including ADNI-MEM [29], ADNI-LAN, ADNI-EF, and ADNI-VS (https://adni.bitbucket.io/reference/docs/UWNPSYCHSUM/adni_uwnpsychsum_doc_20200326.pdf) [30]. In A05, we generated composites (i.e., A05-MEM, A05-LAN, A05-EF, A05-VS) based on available cognitive tests using a pre-established approach (see Additional file 1: Methods, Table S1) [31, 32]. For each cognitive composite and cohort, annual cognitive changes were determined by fitting linear mixed models with cognitive composites as the dependent variable and time (i.e., years from baseline) as the independent variable, with subject-specific random slope and intercept [10, 33]. Thereby, we derived subject-specific annual cognitive change rates for each cognitive domain.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were computed using R statistical software version 4.0.2 (https://www.R-project.org/).

Differences in baseline characteristics between diagnostic groups were tested using ANOVAs for continuous and chi-squared tests for categorical data.

To compare the accuracy of different tau-PET ROIs (i.e., cognitive-domain-specific/global/temporal, Fig. 1B) for predicting cognitive decline, we performed bootstrapped linear regression with 1000 iterations per cognitive domain and tau-PET ROI. Within each iteration, cognitive changes on MEM/LAN/EF/VS were included as dependent variables, and tau-PET ROIs (i.e., cognitive-domain-specific/global/temporal) as independent variables. For ADNI, all models were controlled for age, sex, education, clinical status, APOE4 status, and baseline performance of the respective cognitive composite. For A05, continuous measures of education were not consistently available; hence, models were corrected for age, sex, clinical status, APOE4 status, and baseline performance of the respective cognitive composite. In each iteration, resulting partial R2 values (variance explained in cognitive change) of the respective tau-PET ROI were extracted. The resulting partial R2 distributions of the different ROIs were then compared using paired t-tests and standardized differences were calculated using Cohen’s d. In addition, 95% confidence intervals (CI) of bootstrapped partial R2 distributions were determined for non-parametric comparisons. Main analyses were conducted in AD-spectrum patients (i.e., Aβ+) and exploratorily repeated in the pooled CN Aβ− plus AD-spectrum sample (Fig. 1C (I)).

Next, we aimed to determine patient-specific cognitive composites that are informed by the baseline tau-PET-based prediction of cognitive decline, using ADNI as a discovery sample and A05 as a validation sample. Within ADNI Aβ+, we ran 1000 bootstrapped regressions controlled for age, sex, clinical status, and APOE4 status to extract 1000 beta-values of the association between cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET and cognitive changes (i.e., ADNI-MEM/ADNI-LAN/ADNI-EF/ADNI-VS) for each variable of the regression model (Fig. 1C (II)). Note, that the cognitive composites which were used for building the model (e.g., ADNI-MEM) included slightly differed tests than the cognitive composites of the validation cohort (e.g., A05-MEM), ensuring additional generalizability of the model and independence from specific tests for MEM/LAN/EF/VS.

Next, we entered patient-specific baseline data of both ADNI (i.e., discovery) and A05 (i.e., validation) subjects in the 1000 linear model equations to determine mean patient-specific estimates of cognitive decline. To generate a personalized cognitive composite, we computed squared ranks of predicted cognitive changes across MEM/LAN/EF/VS for each subject (i.e., from 12=slowest predicted decline up to 42=fastest predicted decline), in order to maximize the information weight of cognitive domains with fast predicted cognitive decline. For each subject, cognitive changes (see the “Assessment of cognitive changes” section) were then weighted by the tau-PET informed squared rank (i.e., 12, 22, 32, 42; see Fig. 1C (III)) to determine a weighted, personalized cognitive composite that summarizes cognitive changes based on the degree of tau-PET-predicted cognitive decline. Using a paired t-test, we then tested whether tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composites show stronger cognitive changes compared to conventional cognitive composites that average all cognitive domains.

Lastly, we exploratorily tested whether tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composites are more sensitive for detecting treatment effects compared to conventional measures of global cognition and memory. To that end, we simulated interventions by attenuating cognitive changes by 20/30/40% and calculated required sample sizes for detecting simulated intervention effects using the R-package pwr (settings: two-sample t-test, comparing actual vs. attenuated cognitive changes; two-tailed, alpha = 0.05, power = 0.8; see https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/pwr/pwr.pdf). This analysis was performed for tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composites, an unweighted cognitive composite (i.e., z-score mean across actual change rates of ADNI/A05-MEM/LAN/EF/VS), as well as actual change rates of MEM (ADNI-MEM/A05-MEM) [34]. The latter one was selected, since episodic memory decline is one of the hallmark cognitive symptoms in AD as extensively reported before [35,36,37]. The analyses were repeated in the ADNI and A05 AD spectrum groups excluding APOE4 status as a covariate to test whether our models also apply in more simple settings without APOE genotype availability (see Additional file 1 [ADNI: n = 149; A05: n = 67]).

Results

Sample characteristics

We included 183/53/25 CN/MCI/demented ADNI subjects (age = 74.89±7.48 years) and 51/35/25 CN/MCI/demented A05 individuals (age = 71.29±10.03 years). Descriptive statistics stratified by cohort, clinical status, and amyloid status are shown in Table 1. Group-mean tau-PET SUVRs are shown in Additional file 1: Fig. S1.

Table 1 Subjects characteristics

Cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET outperforms conventional tau-PET metrics for predicting domain-specific cognitive decline

First, we determined the accuracy of cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET vs. global and temporal-lobe tau-PET for predicting future decline in MEM/LAN/EF/VS. In ADNI Aβ+, bootstrapped linear regression revealed cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET as a better predictor of cognitive decline than global/temporal tau-PET for MEM (cognitive-domain-specific/global/temporal tau-PET: partial R2 = 0.175/0.108/0.152; Fig. 2A), LAN (cognitive-domain-specific/global/temporal tau-PET: partial R2 = 0.201/0.164/0.136; Fig. 2B), EF (cognitive-domain-specific/global/temporal tau-PET: partial R2 = 0.132/0.088/0.029; Fig. 2C), and VS (cognitive-domain-specific/global/temporal tau-PET: partial R2 = 0.192/0.097/0.070; Fig. 2D). Further, 95%CIs did not overlap between bootstrapped partial R2-distributions of the cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET and global/temporal tau-PET ROIs, providing non-parametric support that cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET explains more variance in cognitive decline than conventional tau-PET measures (Fig. 2A–D). Congruent results were obtained for the A05 Aβ+ validation cohort for all cognitive tests (Fig. 2E–H). Detailed results of bootstrapped regressions are provided in Additional file 1: Tables S2&3. When repeating these analyses in the pooled CN Aβ− plus Aβ+ sample, we obtained largely congruent results in both cohorts except for LAN in A05 (Additional file 1: Table S3). Using SUVRs rather than Gaussian mixture model transformed tau-PET signal of the cognitive-domain-specific ROI yielded consistent results except for ADNI-MEM and ADNI/A05-LAN (see Additional file 1: Table S4&5). The results remained consistent when excluding APOE4 status as covariate from the model (Additional file 1: Table S6-10 and Figure S2). Together, these findings suggest that combining tau-PET with meta-analytical task-fMRI maps of major cognitive functions improves the prediction of domain-specific cognitive decline compared to conventional global/temporal-lobe tau-PET measures in AD.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Bootstrapped linear models (1000 iterations) revealed that partial R2 values (explained variance in annual cognitive changes) were higher for cognitive-domain-specific tau than for global or temporal tau-PET. The boxplots illustrate the partial R2 distributions for episodic memory, language, executive functioning, and visuospatial abilities within the AD-spectrum (ADNI cohort: AD; A05 cohort: EH). The models were controlled for age, sex [in ADNI: education], clinical status, baseline score of the respective cognitive test, and APOE4 status. R2-distributions across different regions of interests (global vs. temporal vs. cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET) were compared with paired t-tests. 95% confidence intervals for the mean are displayed as horizontal lines. Within each panel, the tau-PET ROI with the highest explained variance is highlighted by an underscore. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001

A tau-PET-informed cognitive composite increases the sensitivity to assess cognitive decline

Next, we used tau-PET-based prediction of cognitive decline to determine personalized cognitive composites to better capture individual tau-related cognitive trajectories. Specifically, we ran 1000 bootstrapped regression models in ADNI Aβ+ (i.e., discovery sample) to determine beta-estimates for the association between cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET and cognitive decline, controlling for age, sex, clinical status, and APOE4 status. To derive patient-specific estimates for cognitive decline in ADNI Aβ+, we then entered individual tau-PET measures and covariates in bootstrapped linear model equations to determine mean estimates for cognitive decline. For each subject, we then square-ranked the resulting cognitive change rates on the four cognitive domains (i.e., from 12=slowest predicted cognitive decline up to 42=fastest predicted cognitive decline), to determine a rank-weighted cognitive composite that maximizes the information-weight of those cognitive domains in which cognitive decline is expected based on baseline tau-PET. The same linear model equations derived from ADNI Aβ+ were then applied to A05 Aβ+ for validation. In addition, we determined an “unweighted” cognitive composite as the average z-score across all four cognitive domains. We found that the tau-PET-informed patient-specific cognitive composite showed faster longitudinal decline than the unweighted cognitive composite in ADNI Aβ+ (T = 20.442, p < 0.001, Cohen’s dpaired = 1.728) but not in A05 Aβ+ (T = −1.423, p = 0.160, Cohen’s dpaired = 0.176; Fig. 3). When repeating the analysis excluding APOE4 status as covariate, we found a faster longitudinal decline of the tau-PET-informed patient-specific cognitive composite in ADNI (T = 15.624, p < 0.001, Cohen’s dpaired = 1.280) and in A05 (T = 2.758, p = 0.008, Cohen’s dpaired = 0.337; Additional file 1: Figure S3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Comparison of unweighted (i.e., average of MEM/LAN/EF/VS) and tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composites for AD-spectrum patients (i.e., Aβ+) within the ADNI cohort (A) and the A05 cohort (B). Statistics were derived from paired t-tests. The model for the tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composites was controlled for age, sex, clinical status, and APOE4 status

Patient-centered cognitive composites can reduce the sample sizes to detect treatment effects in clinical trials

Lastly, we exploratorily assessed whether using tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composites as an endpoint in clinical trials increases the sensitivity to detect intervention effects. To this end, we compared estimated sample sizes for simulated intervention effects (i.e., 20/30/40% attenuated cognitive decline) for different cognitive endpoints in Aβ+. Specifically, we calculated estimated sample sizes for MEM (ADNI-MEM/A05-MEM), as well as unweighted (i.e., mean across ADNI/A05-MEM/LAN/VIS/EF) and tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composites. Across both the ADNI discovery and A05 validation sample, tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composites required the lowest number of participants to detect intervention effects for all intervention strengths, although effects were stronger in ADNI (Table 2). Although no significant differences were found between the unweighted and the tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composite in A05, our finding that the personalized cognitive composite required the lowest sample sizes also in the A05 cohort suggests that personalized cognitive composites assess cognitive change over time more sensitively than cognitive composites that are agnostic for patient-specific tau-dependent cognitive trajectories. When repeating the analysis without APOE4 as covariate, the results remained consistent, showing the lowest required sample sizes when applying the personalized cognitive composite for both cohorts (Additional file 1: Table 11).

Table 2 Sample size estimation to detect simulated intervention effects of 20%, 30%, and 40% in AD-spectrum patients. The model for the tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composites was controlled for age, sex, clinical status, and APOE4 status

Discussion

Our first finding was that mapping baseline tau-PET to meta-analytical task-fMRI brain activation maps of MEM/LAN/EF/VS facilitates personalized prediction of tau-related cognitive trajectories in AD. Specifically, we found tau-PET in cognitive-domain-specific brain regions to predict domain-specific cognitive decline better than conventional tau-PET metrics (i.e., global/temporal-lobe tau-PET) which have been previously shown to be prognostic markers in AD [9, 10]. Second, we showed that using cognitive-domain-specific tau-PET as an information source to determine patient-tailored cognitive composites improves the sensitivity to assess AD-related cognitive decline compared to conventional cognitive composites. In exploratory simulated interventions, we showed further that using tau-PET-informed personalized composites as cognitive endpoints may reduce sample sizes required to detect tau-targeting treatment effects compared to conventional endpoints (e.g., MEM, global composites). Together, our independently validated findings suggest that combining tau-PET with fMRI-based mapping of cognitive abilities can facilitate the prediction of AD-related cognitive trajectories, which may improve patient-specific assessments of cognitive changes in clinical trials that target tau pathology.

First, we reported that applying task-fMRI maps of MEM/LAN/EF/VS to tau-PET in AD patients improves the prediction of cognitive-domain-specific decline compared to conventional prognostic tau-PET readouts [9, 10]. We specifically focused on these cognitive domains since they are typically assessed in cognitive test protocols for evaluating AD in standard clinical settings and clinical trials [30, 38, 39]. Our findings critically extend previous cross-sectional AD studies, emphasizing a close link between spatially heterogeneous patterns of tau deposition and neurodegeneration with heterogeneous symptom manifestation [14, 15, 40]. Since tau pathology has been shown to disrupt neuronal connectivity in preclinical [41] and clinical studies [42,43,44,45], neurotoxic tau may drive network dysfunction [46] and impairment in the cognitive domain that is supported by the tau-affected network. Supporting this, we found that higher tau-PET in brain regions that support a given cognitive domain [17, 18] is associated with faster decline in that cognitive domain. While the current study investigated tau-PET as a predictor of cognitive decline for a selected set of cognitive domains typically affected in AD [47] our proposed methodological framework can be applied for mapping tau-PET to other cognitive or non-cognitive domains, which may motivate future efforts to investigate tau-PET for predicting patient-centered disease trajectories in AD.

Second, we show that combining baseline tau-PET with task-fMRI maps of MEM/LAN/EF/VS allows determining personalized cognitive composites with increased sensitivity to detect patient-specific cognitive decline compared to conventional patient-agnostic cognitive composites. Specifically, we used baseline tau-PET signal in cognitive-domain-specific brain regions to forecast domain-specific cognitive decline. To compute a personalized composite, the actual cognitive change rates with faster tau-PET-predicted decline were weighted higher than actual change rates with slower tau-PET-predicted cognitive decline. Such patient-specific cognitive composites take into account inter-individual variability to facilitate longitudinal assessment of heterogeneous cognitive trajectories [47,48,49] and may thus be applied as personalized endpoints in clinical trials [7, 19]. Supporting this, we exploratorily performed simulated trials in which tau-PET-informed personalized cognitive composites increased the sensitivity to detect treatment effects compared to conventional composites, which have been previously used as endpoints in clinical trials [8, 50]. However, it should be noted that effects on estimated sample sizes for simulated intervention effects were smaller in A05 than in ADNI, potentially due to differences in cognitive tests used to obtain cognitive composites, differences in baseline scores, differences in the overall clinical characteristics between datasets, differences in frequency of amnestic versus non-amnestic phenotypes, APOE4 frequencies, or different data processing strategies. Despite small effect sizes, results were consistent across both samples; hence, our approach for assessing personalized composites may be a starting point for improving the assessment of heterogeneous cognitive trajectories to reduce sample sizes in clinical trials. Nevertheless, it will be important to replicate our findings across larger studies with harmonized data assessment and clinical characteristics.

A clear strength of the present study is the independent validation across two cohorts with different cognitive assessment protocols. Although sample sizes were smaller in the A05 cohort and effects less strong than in ADNI, the congruent results between both cohorts highlighted the robustness of our findings. Nevertheless, several limitations should be highlighted. First, flortaucipir shows off-target binding within hippocampal and subcortical areas [22]. Although these regions were a priori excluded, and Gaussian-mixture modeling was performed to further eliminate off-target binding [24, 25], our results warrant further replication with second-generation tau-PET with an improved off-target binding profile. Second, brain regions like the hippocampus are critical for cognitive function (e.g., memory); hence, second-generation tau-PET which allows hippocampal assessments [51] may further improve the prediction of cognitive decline beyond what is currently possible with flortaucipir. Third, all analyses were determined across the entire AD spectrum. Here, it will be critical to investigate the predictive accuracy of tau-PET for cognitive decline across different clinical groups once larger datasets become available. This will be particularly important for assessing the potential of tau-PET for predicting cognitive decline in patients with early AD. Fourth, the included samples show differences in the level of clinical impairment, i.e., the A05 shows lower performance on MMSE than the ADNI sample, which may confound the tau-PET-based prediction when applying the ADNI-trained models to A05 data. Here, it will be an important next step to train and validate our models across datasets which are fully comparable across all measures.

Conclusion

In conclusion, we demonstrate independently validated evidence for tau-PET combined with fMRI-based mapping of cognitive functions as a promising tool for individualized prediction of cognitive decline in AD. Tau-PET-informed cognitive composites may facilitate detecting intervention effects and thus reduce required sample sizes in clinical trials in AD, which also may be retrospectively assessed in clinical trial data with available tau-PET.

Availability of data and materials

All data used in this manuscript are publicly available from the ADNI database (adni.loni.usc.edu) upon registration and compliance with the data use agreement. The data that support the findings of this study are available on reasonable request from the corresponding author.

Abbreviations

Aβ:

Amyloid-beta

ADNI:

Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative

EF:

Executive functioning composite score

LAN:

Language composite score

MEM:

Episodic memory composite score

VS:

Visuospatial composite score

MMSE:

Mini-Mental State Examination

A05:

AVID-1451-A05 validation cohort

ROI:

Region of interest

SUVR:

Standardized uptake value ratio

References

  1. Fleisher AS, Chen K, Quiroz YT, Jakimovich LJ, Gutierrez Gomez M, Langois CM, et al. Associations between biomarkers and age in the presenilin 1 E280A autosomal dominant Alzheimer disease kindred: a cross-sectional study. JAMA Neurol. 2015;72(3):316–24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Jack CR Jr, Bennett DA, Blennow K, Carrillo MC, Dunn B, Haeberlein SB, et al. NIA-AA research framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(4):535–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Dubois B, Villain N, Frisoni GB, Rabinovici GD, Sabbagh M, Cappa S, et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations of the international working group. Lancet Neurol. 2021;20(6):484–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Komarova NL, Thalhauser CJ. High degree of heterogeneity in Alzheimer’s disease progression patterns. PLoS Comput Biol. 2011;7(11):e1002251.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Vogel JW, Young AL, Oxtoby NP, Smith R, Ossenkoppele R, Strandberg OT, et al. Four distinct trajectories of tau deposition identified in Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Med. 2021;27(5):871–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Ballard C, Atri A, Boneva N, Cummings JL, Frolich L, Molinuevo JL, et al. Enrichment factors for clinical trials in mild-to-moderate Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2019;5:164–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Devi G, Scheltens P. Heterogeneity of Alzheimer’s disease: consequence for drug trials? Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018;10(1):122.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Swanson CJ, Zhang Y, Dhadda S, Wang J, Kaplow J, Lai RYK, et al. A randomized, double-blind, phase 2b proof-of-concept clinical trial in early Alzheimer’s disease with lecanemab, an anti-Abeta protofibril antibody. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2021;13(1):80.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Ossenkoppele R, Smith R, Mattsson-Carlgren N, Groot C, Leuzy A, Strandberg O, et al. Accuracy of tau positron emission tomography as a prognostic marker in preclinical and prodromal Alzheimer disease: a head-to-head comparison against amyloid positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. JAMA Neurol. 2021;78(8):961–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Biel D, Brendel M, Rubinski A, Buerger K, Janowitz D, Dichgans M, et al. Tau-PET and in vivo Braak-staging as prognostic markers of future cognitive decline in cognitively normal to demented individuals. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2021;13(1):137.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Bucci M, Chiotis K, Nordberg A, Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging I. Alzheimer’s disease profiled by fluid and imaging markers: tau PET best predicts cognitive decline. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26(10):5888–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Pontecorvo MJ, Devous MD, Kennedy I, Navitsky M, Lu M, Galante N, et al. A multicentre longitudinal study of flortaucipir (18F) in normal ageing, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Brain. 2019;142(6):1723–35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Lu M, Pontecorvo MJ, Devous MD Sr, Arora AK, Galante N, McGeehan A, et al. Aggregated tau measured by visual interpretation of flortaucipir positron emission tomography and the associated risk of clinical progression of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease: results from 2 phase III clinical trials. JAMA Neurol. 2021;78(4):445–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Ossenkoppele R, Schonhaut DR, Scholl M, Lockhart SN, Ayakta N, Baker SL, et al. Tau PET patterns mirror clinical and neuroanatomical variability in Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2016;139(Pt 5):1551–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Bejanin A, Schonhaut DR, La Joie R, Kramer JH, Baker SL, Sosa N, et al. Tau pathology and neurodegeneration contribute to cognitive impairment in Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2017;140(12):3286–300.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Devous MD Sr, Fleisher AS, Pontecorvo MJ, Lu M, Siderowf A, Navitsky M, et al. Relationships between cognition and neuropathological tau in Alzheimer’s disease assessed by 18F flortaucipir PET. J Alzheimers Dis. 2021;80(3):1091–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat Methods. 2011;8(8):665–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Rubin TN, Koyejo O, Gorgolewski KJ, Jones MN, Poldrack RA, Yarkoni T. Decoding brain activity using a large-scale probabilistic functional-anatomical atlas of human cognition. PLoS Comput Biol. 2017;13(10):e1005649.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Iwashyna TJ, Deane AM. Individualizing endpoints in randomized clinical trials to better inform individual patient care: the TARGET proposal. Crit Care. 2016;20(1):218.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Feldman HH, Fox NC, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):270–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr, Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):263–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Lemoine L, Leuzy A, Chiotis K, Rodriguez-Vieitez E, Nordberg A. Tau positron emission tomography imaging in tauopathies: the added hurdle of off-target binding. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2018;10:232–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Leuzy A, Chiotis K, Lemoine L, Gillberg PG, Almkvist O, Rodriguez-Vieitez E, et al. Tau PET imaging in neurodegenerative tauopathies-still a challenge. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24(8):1112–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Franzmeier N, Dewenter A, Frontzkowski L, Dichgans M, Rubinski A, Neitzel J, et al. Patient-centered connectivity-based prediction of tau pathology spread in Alzheimer’s disease. Sci Adv. 2020;6(48):eabd1327.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Vogel JW, Iturria-Medina Y, Strandberg OT, Smith R, Levitis E, Evans AC, et al. Spread of pathological tau proteins through communicating neurons in human Alzheimer's disease. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):2612.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 1991;82(4):239–59.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Scholl M, Lockhart SN, Schonhaut DR, O'Neil JP, Janabi M, Ossenkoppele R, et al. PET imaging of tau deposition in the aging human brain. Neuron. 2016;89(5):971–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Jack CR Jr, Wiste HJ, Schwarz CG, Lowe VJ, Senjem ML, Vemuri P, et al. Longitudinal tau PET in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2018;141(5):1517–28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Crane PK, Carle A, Gibbons LE, Insel P, Mackin RS, Gross A, et al. Development and assessment of a composite score for memory in the Alzheimer, Äôs Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). Brain Imaging Behavior. 2012;6(4):502–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Choi SE, Mukherjee S, Gibbons LE, Sanders RE, Jones RN, Tommet D, et al. Development and validation of language and visuospatial composite scores in ADNI. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2020;6(1):e12072.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Malek-Ahmadi M, Chen K, Perez SE, He A, Mufson EJ. Cognitive composite score association with Alzheimer’s disease plaque and tangle pathology. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018;10(1):90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Langbaum JB, Hendrix SB, Ayutyanont N, Chen K, Fleisher AS, Shah RC, et al. An empirically derived composite cognitive test score with improved power to track and evaluate treatments for preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(6):666–74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Ewers M, Biechele G, Suarez-Calvet M, Sacher C, Blume T, Morenas-Rodriguez E, et al. Higher CSF sTREM2 and microglia activation are associated with slower rates of beta-amyloid accumulation. EMBO Mol Med. 2020;12(9):e12308.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Gallagher M, Koh MT. Episodic memory on the path to Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2011;21(6):929–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Dekosky ST, Barberger-Gateau P, Cummings J, et al. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6(8):734–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Hampel H, Molinuevo JL, Blennow K, et al. Advancing research diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer’s disease: the IWG-2 criteria. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(6):614–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Crane PK, Carle A, Gibbons LE, Insel P, Mackin RS, Gross A, et al. Development and assessment of a composite score for memory in the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). Brain Imaging Behav. 2012;6(4):502–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Harrison J, Minassian SL, Jenkins L, Black RS, Koller M, Grundman M. A neuropsychological test battery for use in Alzheimer disease clinical trials. Arch Neurol. 2007;64(9):1323–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Tetreault AM, Phan T, Orlando D, Lyu I, Kang H, Landman B, et al. Network localization of clinical, cognitive, and neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2020;143(4):1249–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Busche MA, Wegmann S, Dujardin S, Commins C, Schiantarelli J, Klickstein N, et al. Tau impairs neural circuits, dominating amyloid-beta effects, in Alzheimer models in vivo. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22(1):57–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Liu Y, Yu C, Zhang X, Liu J, Duan Y, Alexander-Bloch AF, et al. Impaired long distance functional connectivity and weighted network architecture in Alzheimer’s disease. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24(6):1422–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Chhatwal JP, Schultz AP, Johnson KA, Hedden T, Jaimes S, Benzinger TLS, et al. Preferential degradation of cognitive networks differentiates Alzheimer's disease from ageing. Brain. 2018;141(5):1486–500.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Kocagoncu E, Quinn A, Firouzian A, Cooper E, Greve A, Gunn R, et al. Tau pathology in early Alzheimer’s disease is linked to selective disruptions in neurophysiological network dynamics. Neurobiol Aging. 2020;92:141–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Schultz AP, Chhatwal JP, Hedden T, Mormino EC, Hanseeuw BJ, Sepulcre J, et al. Phases of hyperconnectivity and hypoconnectivity in the default mode and salience networks track with amyloid and tau in clinically normal individuals. J Neurosci. 2017;37(16):4323–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O. Rich-club organization of the human connectome. J Neurosci. 2011;31(44):15775–86.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Weintraub S, Wicklund AH, Salmon DP. The neuropsychological profile of Alzheimer disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. 2012;2(4):a006171.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Jutten RJ, Sikkes SAM, Van der Flier WM, Scheltens P, Visser PJ, Tijms BM, et al. Finding treatment effects in Alzheimer trials in the face of disease progression heterogeneity. Neurology. 2021;96(22):e2673–e84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Blanken AE, Jang JY, Ho JK, Edmonds EC, Han SD, Bangen KJ, et al. Distilling heterogeneity of mild cognitive impairment in the National Alzheimer Coordinating Center database using latent profile analysis. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(3):e200413.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Salloway S, Farlow M, McDade E, Clifford DB, Wang G, Llibre-Guerra JJ, et al. A trial of gantenerumab or solanezumab in dominantly inherited Alzheimer's disease. Nat Med. 2021;27(7):1187–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Berron D, Vogel JW, Insel PS, Pereira JB, Xie L, Wisse LEM, et al. Early stages of tau pathology and its associations with functional connectivity, atrophy and memory. Brain. 2021;144(9):2771–83.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Mark A. Minton for supporting this study by facilitating access to the A05 data.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. The study was funded by grants from the LMU (FöFoLe, 1032, awarded to NF; FöFoLe, 1119, awarded to DB), the Hertie foundation for clinical neurosciences (awarded to NF), the SyNergy excellence cluster (EXC 2145/ID 390857198), and the German Research Foundation (DFG, INST 409/193-1 FUGG).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Contributions

D.B.: study concept and design, data processing, statistical analysis, interpretation of the results, and writing of the manuscript. Y.L.: data processing and critical revision of the manuscript. M.B.: critical revision of the manuscript. P.H.: data processing and critical revision of the manuscript. A.D.: critical revision of the manuscript. A.M.: critical revision of the manuscript. D.O.: critical revision of the manuscript. I.H.: critical revision of the manuscript. M.P.: critical revision of the manuscript. S.R.: critical revision of the manuscript. A.S.: critical revision of the manuscript. A.R.: critical revision of the manuscript. L.Z.: critical revision of the manuscript. M.S.: critical revision of the manuscript. S.S.: critical revision of the manuscript. M.E.: critical revision of the manuscript. N.F.: study concept and design, data processing, statistical analysis, interpretation of the results, and writing of the manuscript. ADNI provided all data used for this study. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicolai Franzmeier.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics approval was obtained by the ADNI investigators from the local ethical committees of all involved sites. Access to all ADNI data was granted to the investigators of the current study after registration to ADNI (https://adni.loni.usc.edu) and compliance with the data usage agreement. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and all study participants provided written informed consent for data collection. All work complied with ethical regulations for work with human participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Data used in preparation of this article were obtained from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database (adni.loni.usc.edu). As such, the investigators within the ADNI contributed to the design and implementation of ADNI and/or provided data but did not participate in the analysis or writing of this report. A complete listing of ADNI investigators can be found at: http://adni.loni.usc.edu/wp-content/uploads/how_to_apply/ADNI_Acknowledgement_List.pdf

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1. 

Additional methods and results.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Biel, D., Luan, Y., Brendel, M. et al. Combining tau-PET and fMRI meta-analyses for patient-centered prediction of cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Alz Res Therapy 14, 166 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-022-01105-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-022-01105-5

Keywords