Skip to main content

Societal and equity challenges for Brain Health Services. A user manual for Brain Health Services—part 6 of 6

This article has been updated

Abstract

Brain Health Services are a novel approach to the personalized prevention of dementia. In this paper, we consider how such services can best reflect their social, cultural, and economic context and, in doing so, deliver fair and equitable access to risk reduction. We present specific areas of challenge associated with the social context for dementia prevention. The first concentrates on how Brain Health Services engage with the “at-risk“ individual, recognizing the range of factors that shape an individual’s risk of dementia and the efficacy of risk reduction measures. The second emphasizes the social context of Brain Health Services themselves and their ability to provide equitable access to risk reduction. We then elaborate proposals for meeting or mitigating these challenges. We suggest that considering these challenges will enable Brain Health Services to address two fundamental questions: the balance between an individualized “high-risk” and population focus for public health prevention and the ability of services to meet ethical standards of justice and health equity.

Background

The development and implementation of “precision” Brain Health Services (BHSs) represent a novel approach to reducing the risk of dementia in older adults. In this paper, we consider how such services can best reflect their social, cultural, and economic context and, in doing so, deliver fair and equitable access to risk reduction. Our aim is to provide a constructive critique and initiate discussion around the challenges faced and raised by brain health programs.

We focus on the model of the BHS as developed by Frisoni et al. [1]. BHSs set out a model of personalized prevention, based around individualized multidomain interventions and educational activities and, in anticipation of potential disease-modifying therapies, around reshaped secondary care services. Whereas those with objective cognitive impairment are often served by existing memory clinics, BHSs extend to those without objective cognitive impairment but are concerned they are at higher risk of developing dementia for a potentially varying set of reasons, including family history. BHSs feature specific organization, structure and challenges [2], and have four main missions (extensively discussed in the pertinent papers published in this issue of Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy): dementia risk profiling [3], dementia risk communication [4], dementia risk reduction [5], and cognitive enhancement [6].

Our discussion proceeds as follows. First, we consider the practical challenges associated with the social and economic context for the delivery of BHSs. In each section, we frame these around a specific challenge and suggest potential practical, policy and research responses. In closing, we argue that situating BHSs in their social context requires attention to the ability of such services to deliver access to risk reduction in a socially just and equitable fashion.

We present two specific areas of challenge associated with the social context for dementia prevention. The first concentrates on how BHSs engage with the “at-risk” individual, recognizing the range of factors that shape an individual’s risk of dementia and the efficacy of risk reduction measures. The second emphasizes the social context of BHSs themselves and their ability to provide equitable access to risk reduction. Rather than simply pointing out challenges, however, we introduce proposals for remedying or mitigating them. Considering these challenges would enable BHSs to address two fundamental questions: the balance between a “high-risk” and “population” focus for public health prevention and the ability of services to meet ethical standards of justice and health equity.

The at-risk individual in social context

The social determinants of dementia risk

  • Challenge. A focus on self-referring high-risk individuals within specialist BHSs does not reflect the wider social and economic determinants of brain health and cannot be the most effective and efficient approach to risk reduction at a population level.

  • Elaboration. An individual’s risk of developing dementia is a consequence of their genetic background and the accumulation of factors over the life course [7, 8]. Globally, it is estimated that more than a third of the incidence of dementia in the population is due to exposure to potentially modifiable risk factors. The age-specific incidence of dementia is falling in many countries as a result of improvements in childhood education, nutrition, healthcare, and lifestyle changes [9, 10].

    These findings have clear implications for the promotion of brain health. The Lancet Commission focuses recommendations on 12 modifiable risk factors: the treatment of hypertension, childhood education, exercise, social engagement, smoking and alcohol consumption, hearing loss, depression, diabetes, obesity, air pollution, and traumatic brain injury [8]. Many of these risk factors may be amenable to behavior change and lifestyle approaches. However, among them are those that are not easily incorporated into an approach to brain health focused on the individual rather than populations or groups—such as access to education in early life or exposure to air pollution. In fact, even factors which are often considered as “individually” modifiable are unequally distributed, are closely associated with wider forms of social, economic, health, and gender inequality, and are grounded in social and economic conditions that run through the life course [8, 11, 12]. Importantly, they also reflect structurally and institutionally embedded inequalities and discrimination related to race/ethnicity—both in the social distribution of risk factors, such as access to education, and the representation of diverse populations in clinical and epidemiological datasets [13, 14].

    The implications for BHSs are twofold. First, primary prevention activities in populations have already been shown to have significant societal value. Thinking how to develop these sensibly within contemporary populations would be a worthwhile focus for research and policy. Second, a “high-risk” focus needs to engage with social and economic determinants of dementia risk, and their uneven distribution across populations, as this is where the greatest gain may be achieved in terms of future risk reduction.

  • Solution. Individualized approaches to brain health need to explicitly account for the social and economic distribution of risk across the life course in order to shift population risk profiles for cognitive decline and dementia at each life stage [15]. All BHSs should commit to programs that are not only individual risk reduction programs, but that aim to address economic and social determinants of health and disease by empowering individuals and communities who are at greatest disadvantage.

Risk communication and behavior change

  • Challenge. The concept of BHSs relies largely on individualized risk prediction, using models derived from a combination of lifestyle, genetic, and biomarker information. This information forms the basis for personalized “precision” prevention plans. It is essential, however, that these interventions recognize the psycho-social challenges and complexity associated with behavior change and acknowledge the uncertainties associated with risk prediction models.

  • Elaboration. The use of genetic, biomarker, and lifestyle information to identify “high-risk” individuals for “secondary prevention” is central to the BHS model. The use of this information to make significant differences to individual risk, however, remains understudied. Here, there is potential to learn from risk reduction programs in other clinical areas. The central message of systematic reviews of this work has been that the individualized provision of risk information alone has neither strong nor consistent effects on health-related behaviors [16]. In the case of cardiovascular disease, it has been shown that personalized risk information can improve the accuracy of an individual’s perception of their risk and may have implications for improving clinical prescribing but, again, has little effect on the way individuals live their lives [17]. In the case of genetic risk information, trials of the provision of genetic risk information have shown no or very limited effects on health behaviors [18].

    For dementia itself, there is also a need to be cognizant of the uncertainties associated with risk information. These uncertainties reflect the limitations of the evidence base that informs current prediction models when applied to individuals and have consequences for those acting upon them [19]. Thus, for example, there is evidence of differing effects of ancestry on genetic risk but also significant under-representation of racially and ethnically diverse populations in studies of imaging and genomics, as well as clinical trials [20,21,22,23]. Uncertainties are likely to be particularly exacerbated among those attending BHSs from the general population, rather than traditional memory clinic populations or among those with a diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment [24,25,26].

    Sub-analyses of the FINGER and MAPT studies suggest that a multimodal programs of risk reduction may have some, limited in size, value for high-risk individuals, particularly those at greater genetic risk (i.e., APOE ɛ4 carriers) [27] or with brain amyloidosis [28]. However, these were not the primary endpoints and such findings must be confirmed in targeted studies. Together, the current risk reduction interventions and the apparently limited value of approaches based solely on risk communication suggest there is an opportunity for work on dementia prevention to learn from, rather than repeat, the mistakes of other fields. To make a significant difference, BHSs should have a robust model of the complexity of health behaviors and their change [29].

  • Solution. Approaches to risk reduction that rely on individualized risk prediction leading to behavior changes are likely to be of limited effectiveness. Indeed, the vast bulk of evidence about changing behavior takes account of the fact that much decision making is automatic, and that changing behavior involves a combination of capability, motivation, and opportunity [30]. This may include changes to the environment to enhance the capacity for healthy behaviors to occur. For example, there is some evidence that approaches to CVD risk reduction focusing on a range of community based approaches may not only be efficacious but also reduce inequalities in health outcomes [31, 32]. In the case of dementia prevention, this may include the creation of life-course environments that encourage physical activity, good nutrition, educational opportunities, social engagement, and a healthy physical environment.

    In engaging with uncertainty, the actual nature of the evidence base, rather than the hyperbole, for prediction models and individualized risk reduction programs should be discussed with members of the public and patients who seek such approaches, in relation to their age, gender, and race/ethnicity, with regard to short-, medium-, and long-term prospects of benefit, known harms, and likely cost.

The BHSs in a social context

Access to BHSs

  • Challenge. Specialist BHSs risk compounding issues of access associated with existing clinical services, limiting their value to disadvantaged populations.

  • Elaboration. Expanding services targeting the “worried well” risk reinforcing the “inverse care law,” targeting those already well-served by health care and preventative health services, many of whom would probably have relatively little likelihood of benefitting and are probably most likely to have optimal brain health [33]. Where these services are reimbursed, it is important to consider the trade-offs with services that are not being implemented, with better evidence bases that might achieve more for more people [34]. Where private health care providers offer services, potential customers who feel they may be beneficial should be informed about the state of evidence and its limitations. The recent history of direct-to-consumer genetic testing and “brain training” tools shows that there are likely to be many products and services whose marketing may lead individuals to assume a health benefit that is not justified by the evidence base [35, 36].

    In addition, interventions that require an individual to draw on their own resources—whether social, economic, or psychological—tend to disproportionately benefit those with more of these resources [37]. This can result in the accumulation of disadvantage—both through exposure to risk, as discussed above, and in terms of access to testing and diagnostic services.

    Access to memory clinics, and even dementia services, is unequally distributed within and between countries, and is low around the world, varying significantly by country. Some such inequalities are a result of the geographical distribution of services within countries, particularly where they are concentrated in urban areas [38, 39]. People living in rural areas may thus have less access to specialist dementia services and post-diagnostic support, either because they do not exist or are inaccessible by public transport, and there may be a greater emphasis on the role of primary care [40].

    Further, in the UK and USA, access to a diagnosis has been shown to be associated with older age, male gender, and higher level of education and socio-economic status including income [41,42,43]. Those from less deprived socio-economic groups are more likely to be initiated on anti-dementia drugs than the most deprived and may also present at clinic earlier in the progression of symptoms [44, 45]. Race/ethnicity is also associated with access. In the USA, those from non-Hispanic Black and Hispanic populations are more likely to have a missed or delayed diagnosis, less likely to access diagnostic services later, and be less likely to access medication or research trials [46,47,48]. Similar findings have been found in the UK for Asian men and women, and Black men [43, 49, 50]. Importantly though, data on race/ethnicity is often not collected—in a study based on a representative sample of UK primary care records, race/ethnicity was far more likely to be missing from records than information on deprivation [49]. These lacunae in the data present challenges in understanding who is accessing, and who is excluded, from BHSs, and further exacerbate the biases in data discussed above.

    Further, access to BHSs is likely to be shaped by cultural framings of both dementia and risk. Available evidence suggests cultural factors that shape access to memory clinics, and, while specific evidence is lacking, it seems likely they would similarly affect the distribution of those who attend BHSs. Such cultural factors may differ by country and between racial or ethnic groups. Qualitative research, for example, suggests potential cultural differences related to the recognition or acceptance of dementia, or hesitations in seeking a diagnosis among minority ethnic groups, such that medical help may not be sought and dementia seen as a private problem, associated with significant social stigma [51,52,53,54]. In the USA, public expectations related to learning dementia risk may also reflect concerns about employment and insurance discrimination, considerations that, as Frisoni et al. rightly point out, impact on the decision to communicate risk status. Such considerations also shape how people access services [55].

    To ensure that BHSs address and do not exacerbate inequalities in access, we need robust trial evidence before extending reimbursed BHSs to asymptomatic individuals. The overall benefit to societies of targeted early detection services has not yet been proven in clinical trials and the only international study of screening did not reveal any tangible benefits [34]. It is important to acknowledge the existing clinical context for memory clinics that themselves were not developed out of an evidence base but out of a need to identify individuals at an earlier stage of dementia or cognitive impairment from which to recruit to trials.

    Here again, the development of the BHSs might usefully draw on the experience of other areas, such as the introduction of routine health checks in the field of cardiovascular disease prevention. The introduction of general health checks for reducing illness or mortality has been evaluated as unlikely to be beneficial [56]. In fact, both health checks and individualized behavior change approaches may have potentially negative impacts on overall population health equity [37, 57]. In contrast, evaluations of interventions in healthy eating suggest that those that focus “upstream,” for example by intervening in price, are likely to reduce inequalities [32]. The development of brain health programs should consider and evaluate the impact of such programs on health inequalities and the potential for alternative approaches that may reduce inequity. This offers the opportunity to better understanding the impact of interventions across social inequalities and to increase the potential for genuinely beneficial interventions that consider whole systems and the complex realities of public health [58]. For example, modeling incorporating an explicit emphasis on equity suggests that targeting health checks to areas of disadvantage—using health care records to identify those at greatest risk of adverse outcomes—may make them more efficacious, beneficial and cost-effective [57].

  • Solution. To be of value, investment or reimbursement for individualized approaches to risk reduction must evaluate the value for those they represent in terms of long-term brain health, where/whether an individualized approach will lead to sufficient risk reduction to be systematically supported, and what the implications are in terms of equity. This includes the routine collection and evaluation of socio-economic data related to the impact of BHSs. It also involves the exploration of interventions whose ambition is to address systemic and population-level challenges and approaches to delivery that engage with the cultural, religious, ethnic, and racial diversity of the populations they serve.

Achieving just brain health

In the preceding sections, we have suggested that the challenges for BHSs, when considered in their social context, are considerable but not insurmountable. These challenges, and the remedies we have proposed, are summarized in Table 1. In closing, we consider the benefits of addressing them.

Table 1 The societal challenges encountered by the Brain Health Service model and accompanying recommendations

The first relates to the balance between individualized “high risk” and population prevention strategies. The former may have more significant impacts on individuals, whereas the latter may have more modest individual impacts but a greater impact across the population. Further, shifting the norm of behaviors across the population may have a subsequent impact on what is considered as “high-risk” behavior. Thus, high-risk individuals may be more likely to take physical exercise or reduce smoking if this is considered to be normal [59]. As Frisoni et al. note, following Rose, high-risk and population strategies are not inherently exclusive [1, 60]. The benefits of population prevention may sit alongside those targeting individuals with pathological changes associated with increased risk of dementia. However, as Rose also recognized, the complementarity of these approaches relies on a lack of competition for resources. In the current environment for healthcare where competition for resources is intense, a resource intensive high-risk approach inevitably limits the possibilities for population measures—even when the overall benefit of the latter may be greater.

The development of BHSs thus requires addressing a wider range of issues than currently considered within the “ethics” of BHSs. Discussion of the ethics of dementia prevention has been dominated by a focus on autonomy, particularly the right of individuals to know, or not to know, their risk [1, 61]. The challenges presented here emphasize the critical and compelling importance of widening this discussion, particularly as the concerns of the memory clinic and clinical ethics encounter those of population health, public health, and societal ethics. It has been recognized that it is essential that efforts to prevent dementia “leave no one behind” [62]. Social justice must be embedded as a core value and guiding principle for brain health programs [63, 64]. This means improving health to improve well-being, by focusing on the needs of the most disadvantaged, to ensure the fair distribution of common advantages and the sharing of common burdens. At heart, following Beauchamp’s framing of public health ethics, it requires thinking about and reacting to the problem of brain health as “primarily collective problems of the entire society” [64].

Conclusions

BHSs that focus on the delivery of personalized risk scores and interventions targeted at “high risk” individuals may have potential as an approach limited to reducing risk in some sections of the population. However, this must be developed hand in glove with improving the means of reducing the overall population burden of dementia through life-course and broader societal measures. It is these measures that will change the incidence and prevalence of dementia, as well illustrated in the last 50 years. It is therefore essential that those who invest in the development of BHSs aimed at individualized services consider the just allocation of resources between approaches. While high-risk and population approaches can be complementary, they risk competing for scarce resources, particularly given the resource-intensiveness of clinical risk assessment and follow-up. Without attention to the factors discussed here, an approach focusing primarily on high-risk populations is likely to struggle to deliver fair and equitable access to services or to risk reduction.

Availability of data and materials

Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.

Change history

  • 23 October 2021

    The paper was amended to add a DOI in references part of the Brain Health Services series.

Abbreviations

BHS:

Brain Health Service

References

  1. Frisoni GB, Molinuevo JL, Altomare D, Carrera E, Barkhof F, Berkhof J, et al. Precision prevention of Alzheimer’s and other dementias: Anticipating future needs in the control of risk factors and implementation of disease-modifying therapies. Alzheimers Dementia. 2020 [cited 2020 Sep 22];n/a. Available from: https://alz-journals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002/alz.12132;16(10):1457–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Altomare D, Molinuevo JL, Ritchie C, Ribaldi F, Carrera E, Dubois B, Jessen F, McWhirter L, Scheltens P, van der Flier WM, Vellas B, Démonet JF, Frisoni GB. Brain Health Services: Organization, structure and challenges for implementation. A user manual for Brain Health Services – Part 1 of 6. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-021-00827-2.

  3. Ranson JM, Rittman T, Hayat S, Brayne C, Jessen F, Blennow K, van Duijn C, Barkhof F, Tang E, Mummery CJ, Stephan BCM, Altomare D, Frisoni GB, Ribaldi F, Molinuevo JL, Scheltens P, Llewellyn, DJ. Modifiable risk factors for dementia and dementia risk profiling. A user manual for Brain Health Services – Part 2 of 6. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-021-00895-4.

  4. Visser LNC, Minguillon C, Sánchez-Benavides G, Abramowicz M, Altomare D, Fauria K, Frisoni GB, Georges J, Ribaldi F, Scheltens P, van der Schaar J, Zwan M, van der Flier WM, Molinuevo JL. Dementia risk communication. A user manual for Brain Health Services – Part 3 of 6. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-021-00840-5.

  5. Solomon A, Stephen R, Altomare D, Carrera E, Frisoni GB, Kulmala J, Molinuevo JL, Nilsson P, Ngandu T, Ribaldi F, Vellas B, Scheltens P, Kivipelto M. Multidomain interventions: state-of-the-art and future directions for protocols to implement precision dementia risk reduction. A user manual for Brain Health Services – Part 4 of 6. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-021-00875-8.

  6. Brioschi Guevara A, Bieler M, Altomare D, Berthier M, Csajka C, Dautricourt S, Démonet JF, Dodich A, Frisoni GB, Miniussi C, Molinuevo JL, Ribaldi F, Scheltens P, Chételat G. Protocols for cognitive enhancement. A user manual for Brain Health Services – Part 5 of 6. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-021-00844-1.

  7. Norton S, Matthews FE, Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Brayne C. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(8):788–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70136-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;0 Available from: https://www.thelancet.com/journals/lancet/article/PIIS0140-6736(20)30367-6/abstract. [cited 2020 Jul 31].

  9. Wu Y-T, Fratiglioni L, Matthews FE, Lobo A, Breteler MMB, Skoog I, et al. Dementia in western Europe: epidemiological evidence and implications for policy making. Lancet Neurol. 2016;15(1):116–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00092-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Wolters FJ, Chibnik LB, Waziry R, Anderson R, Berr C, Beiser A, et al. 27-year time trends in dementia incidence in Europe and the US: the Alzheimer Cohorts Consortium. Neurology. 2020;95(5):e519–31. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000010022.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Marmot M, Allen J, Goldblatt P, Boyce T, McNeish D, Grady M. Fair society, healthy lives: the Marmot Review: strategic review of health inequalities in England post-2010. 2010. London: Department of International Development; 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Russ TC, Stamatakis E, Hamer M, Starr JM, Kivimäki M, Batty GD. Socioeconomic status as a risk factor for dementia death: individual participant meta-analysis of 86 508 men and women from the UK. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203:10–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Weuve J, Barnes LL, Mendes de Leon CF, Rajan KB, Beck T, Aggarwal NT, et al. Cognitive aging in black and white Americans: cognition, cognitive decline, and incidence of Alzheimer disease dementia. Epidemiology. 2018;29(1):151–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0000000000000747.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Barnes LL, Bennett DA. Alzheimer’s disease in african americans: risk factors and challenges for the future. Health Aff. 2014;33(4):580–6. https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.2013.1353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Brayne C, Matthews FE. The determinants of cognitive decline and dementia. BMJ. 2019;366:l4946. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l4946.

  16. French DP, Cameron E, Benton JS, Deaton C, Harvie M. Can communicating personalised disease risk promote healthy behaviour change? A systematic review of systematic reviews. Ann Behav Med. 2017;51(5):718–29. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-017-9895-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Usher-Smith JA, Silarova B, Schuit E, Moons KG, Griffin SJ. Impact of provision of cardiovascular disease risk estimates to healthcare professionals and patients: a systematic review. BMJ Open. 2015;5:e008717.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Hollands GJ, French DP, Griffin SJ, Prevost AT, Sutton S, King S, et al. The impact of communicating genetic risks of disease on risk-reducing health behaviour: systematic review with meta-analysis. BMJ. 2016;352:i1102. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.i1102.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Angehrn Z, Sostar J, Nordon C, Turner A, Gove D, Karcher H, et al. Ethical and Social implications of using predictive modeling for Alzheimer’s disease prevention: a systematic literature review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2020;76:923–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Barnes LL, Arvanitakis Z, Yu L, Kelly J, Jager PLD, Bennett DA. Apolipoprotein E and change in episodic memory in Blacks and Whites. NED. 2013;40:211–9.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Raman R, Quiroz YT, Langford O, Choi J, Ritchie M, Baumgartner M, et al. Disparities by race and ethnicity among adults recruited for a preclinical Alzheimer disease trial. JAMA Netw Open. 2021;4(7):e2114364. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.14364.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Sirugo G, Williams SM, Tishkoff SA. The missing diversity in human genetic studies. Cell. 2019;177(1):26–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.02.048.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Goerdten J, Čukić I, Danso SO, Carrière I, Muniz-Terrera G. Statistical methods for dementia risk prediction and recommendations for future work: a systematic review. Alzheimers Dementia. 2019;5(1):563–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trci.2019.08.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Stephan BCM, Kurth T, Matthews FE, Brayne C, Dufouil C. Dementia risk prediction in the population: are screening models accurate? Nat Rev Neurol. 2010;6(6):318–26. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrneurol.2010.54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Tang EYH, Harrison SL, Errington L, Gordon MF, Visser PJ, Novak G, et al. Current developments in dementia risk prediction modelling: an updated systematic review. PLoS One. 2015;10(9):e0136181. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136181.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. van Maurik IS, Vos SJ, Bos I, Bouwman FH, Teunissen CE, Scheltens P, et al. Biomarker-based prognosis for people with mild cognitive impairment (ABIDE): a modelling study. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18(11):1034–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30283-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Solomon A, Turunen H, Ngandu T, Peltonen M, Levälahti E, Helisalmi S, et al. Effect of the apolipoprotein E genotype on cognitive change during a multidomain lifestyle intervention: a subgroup analysis of a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Neurol. 2018;75(4):462–70. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.4365.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Andrieu S, Guyonnet S, Coley N, Cantet C, Bonnefoy M, Bordes S, et al. Effect of long-term omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation with or without multidomain intervention on cognitive function in elderly adults with memory complaints (MAPT): a randomised, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Neurol. 2017;16(5):377–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30040-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Richard E, Moll van Charante EP, Hoevenaar-Blom MP, Coley N, Barbera M, van der Groep A, et al. Healthy ageing through internet counselling in the elderly (HATICE): a multinational, randomised controlled trial. Lancet Digital Health. 2019;1:e424–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Michie S, van Stralen MM, West R. The behaviour change wheel: a new method for characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. Implement Sci. 2011;6(1):42. https://doi.org/10.1186/1748-5908-6-42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Wendel-Vos GCW, Dutman AE, Verschuren WMM, Ronckers ET, Ament A, van Assema P, et al. Lifestyle factors of a five-year community-intervention program: the Hartslag Limburg intervention. Am J Prev Med. 2009;37(1):50–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2009.03.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. McGill R, Anwar E, Orton L, Bromley H, Lloyd-Williams F, O’Flaherty M, et al. Are interventions to promote healthy eating equally effective for all? Systematic review of socioeconomic inequalities in impact. BMC Public Health. 2015;15(1):457. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-015-1781-7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Hart JT. The Inverse Care Law. Lancet. 1971;297(7696):405–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(71)92410-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Fowler NR, Perkins AJ, Gao S, Sachs GA, Boustani MA. Risks and benefits of screening for dementia in primary care: the Indiana University cognitive health outcomes investigation of the comparative effectiveness of dementia screening (IU CHOICE) trial. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2020;68(3):535–43. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.16247.

  35. Annas GJ, Elias S. 23andMe and the FDA. N Engl J Med. 2014;370(11):985–8. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp1316367.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Schaper M, Schicktanz S. Medicine, market and communication: ethical considerations in regard to persuasive communication in direct-to-consumer genetic testing services. BMC Med Ethics. 2018;19(1):56. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12910-018-0292-3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Capewell S, Graham H. Will cardiovascular disease prevention widen health inequalities? PLoS Med. 2010;7:e1000320.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Cahill S, Pierce M, Moore V. A national survey of memory clinics in the Republic of Ireland. Int Psychogeriatr. 2014;26(4):605–13. https://doi.org/10.1017/S104161021300238X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Morgan DG, Crossley M, Kirk A, D’Arcy C, Stewart N, Biem J, et al. Improving access to dementia care: development and evaluation of a rural and remote memory clinic. null. Routledge. 2009;13:17–30.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Szymczynska P, Innes A, Mason A, Stark C. A review of diagnostic process and postdiagnostic support for people with dementia in rural areas. J Prim Care Community Health. 2011;2(4):262–76. https://doi.org/10.1177/2150131911404705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Savva GM, Arthur A. Who has undiagnosed dementia? A cross-sectional analysis of participants of the Aging, Demographics and Memory Study. Age Ageing. 2015;44(4):642–7. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afv020.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Lang L, Clifford A, Wei L, Zhang D, Leung D, Augustine G, et al. Prevalence and determinants of undetected dementia in the community: a systematic literature review and a meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2017;7(2):e011146. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2016-011146.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Amjad H, Roth DL, Sheehan OC, Lyketsos CG, Wolff JL, Samus QM. Underdiagnosis of dementia: an observational study of patterns in diagnosis and awareness in US older adults. J Gen Intern Med. 2018;33(7):1131–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-018-4377-y.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Cooper C, Lodwick R, Walters K, Raine R, Manthorpe J, Iliffe S, et al. Observational cohort study: deprivation and access to anti-dementia drugs in the UK. Age Ageing. 2016;45(1):148–54. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afv154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Qian W, Schweizer TA, Fischer CE. Impact of socioeconomic status on initial clinical presentation to a memory disorders clinic. Int Psychogeriatr. 2014;26(4):597–603. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610213002299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Cooper C, Tandy AR, Balamurali TBS, Livingston G. A systematic review and meta-analysis of ethnic differences in use of dementia treatment, care, and research. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(3):193–203. https://doi.org/10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181bf9caf.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Lin P-J, Daly AT, Olchanski N, Cohen JT, Neumann PJ, Faul JD, et al. Dementia diagnosis disparities by race and ethnicity. Med Care. 2021;59(8):679–86. https://doi.org/10.1097/MLR.0000000000001577.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Gianattasio KZ, Prather C, Glymour MM, Ciarleglio A, Power MC. Racial disparities and temporal trends in dementia misdiagnosis risk in the United States. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;5:891–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trci.2019.11.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Pham TM, Petersen I, Walters K, Raine R, Manthorpe J, Mukadam N, et al. Trends in dementia diagnosis rates in UK ethnic groups: analysis of UK primary care data. Clin Epidemiol. 2018;10:949–60. https://doi.org/10.2147/CLEP.S152647.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Ogliari G, Turner Z, Khalique J, Gordon AL, Gladman JRF, Chadborn NH. Ethnic disparity in access to the memory assessment service between South Asian and white British older adults in the United Kingdom: a cohort study. Int J Geriatric Psychiatry. 2020;35(5):507–15. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.5263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Mukadam N, Cooper C, Livingston G. Improving access to dementia services for people from minority ethnic groups. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2013;26(4):409–14. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e32835ee668.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Berwald S, Roche M, Adelman S, Mukadam N, Livingston G. Black African and Caribbean British communities’ perceptions of memory problems: “We Don’t Do Dementia”. PLoS One. 2016;11 Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4821595/. [cited 2020 Jul 17].

  53. Mukadam N, Cooper C, Basit B, Livingston G. Why do ethnic elders present later to UK dementia services? A qualitative study. Int Psychogeriatr. 2011;23(7):1070–7. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610211000214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Stites SD, Rubright JD, Karlawish J. What features of stigma do the public most commonly attribute to Alzheimer’s disease dementia? Results of a survey of the U.S. general public. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14:925–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Stites SD, Milne R, Karlawish J. Advances in Alzheimer’s imaging are changing the experience of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;10:285–300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2018.02.006.

  56. Krogsbøll LT, Jørgensen KJ, Gøtzsche PC. General health checks in adults for reducing morbidity and mortality from disease. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2019; [cited 2020 Jul 31]; Available from: https://www.cochranelibrary.com/cdsr/doi/10.1002/14651858.CD009009.pub3/full.

  57. Kypridemos C, Collins B, McHale P, Bromley H, Parvulescu P, Capewell S, et al. Future cost-effectiveness and equity of the NHS Health Check cardiovascular disease prevention programme: microsimulation modelling using data from Liverpool, UK. PLoS Med. 2018;15:e1002573.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Rutter H, Savona N, Glonti K, Bibby J, Cummins S, Finegood DT, et al. The need for a complex systems model of evidence for public health. Lancet. 2017;390(10112):2602–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31267-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. John SD. Risk, contractualism, and Rose’s “prevention paradox”. Soc Theory Pract. 2014;40:28–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Rose G. Sick Individuals and sick populations. Int J Epidemiol. 1985;14(1):32–8. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/14.1.32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Molinuevo JL, Cami J, Carné X, Carrillo MC, Georges J, Isaac MB, et al. Ethical challenges in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease observational studies and trials: results of the Barcelona summit. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12(5):614–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2016.01.009.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Prince M. Progress on dementia—leaving no one behind. Lancet. 2017;390(10113):e51–3. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31757-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Gostin LO, Powers M. What does social justice require for the public’s health? Public Health Ethics And Policy Imperatives. Health Aff. 2006;25(4):1053–60. https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.25.4.1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Beauchamp DE. Public health as social justice. Inquiry. 1976;13:3–14.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

European Task Force for Brain Health Services (in alphabetical order): Marc ABRAMOWICZ, Daniele ALTOMARE, Frederik BARKHOF, Marcelo BERTHIER, Melanie BIELER, Kaj BLENNOW, Carol BRAYNE, Andrea BRIOSCHI, Emmanuel CARRERA, Gael CHÉTELAT, Chantal CSAJKA, Jean-François DEMONET, Alessandra DODICH, Bruno DUBOIS, Giovanni B. FRISONI, Valentina GARIBOTTO, Jean GEORGES, Samia HURST, Frank JESSEN, Miia KIVIPELTO, David LLEWELLYN, Laura McWHIRTER, Richard MILNE, Carolina MINGUILLÓN, Carlo MINIUSSI, José Luis MOLINUEVO, Peter M NILSSON, Janice RANSON, Federica RIBALDI, Craig RITCHIE, Philip SCHELTENS, Alina SOLOMON, Wiesje VAN DER FLIER, Cornelia VAN DUIJN, Bruno VELLAS, Leonie VISSER.

Funding

This paper was the product of a workshop funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation entitled “Dementia Prevention Services” (grant number: IZSEZ0_193593).

GBF received funding by the EU-EFPIA Innovative Medicines Initiatives 2 Joint Undertaking (IMI 2 JU) “European Prevention of Alzheimer’s Dementia consortium” (EPAD, grant agreement number: 115736) and “Amyloid Imaging to Prevent Alzheimer’s Disease” (AMYPAD, grant agreement number: 115952); the Swiss National Science Foundation: “Brain connectivity and metacognition in persons with subjective cognitive decline (COSCODE): correlation with clinical features and in vivo neuropathology” (grant number: 320030_182772).

RM received funding by the EU-EFPIA Innovative Medicines Initiatives 2 Joint Undertaking (IMI 2 JU) “European Prevention of Alzheimer’s Dementia consortium” (EPAD, grant agreement number: 115736) and the Wellcome Trust (grant 206194).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Contributions

Richard Milne and Carol Brayne conceptualized this paper, drafted the manuscript for intellectual content, and approved the manuscript. José Luis Molinuevo revised the manuscript for intellectual content and approved the manuscript. Daniele Altomare, Giovanni B. Frisoni, and Federica Ribaldi conceived and organized the workshop whence the papers of the BHS series in this issue of Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy originated, conceived the related editorial initiative, revised this manuscript for intellectual content, harmonized the manuscript with the other Papers of the BHS series, and approved the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Richard Milne.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

GBF reports grants from Alzheimer Forum Suisse, Académie Suisse des Sciences Médicales, Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Biogen, GE International, Guerbert, Association Suisse pour la Recherche sur l’Alzheimer, IXICO, Merz Pharma, Nestlé, Novartis, Piramal, Roche, Siemens, Teva Pharmaceutical Industries, Vifor Pharma, and Alzheimer’s Association; he has received personal fees from AstraZeneca, Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Elan Pharmaceuticals, GE International, Lundbeck, Pfizer, and TauRx Therapeutics.

The other coauthors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Milne, R., Altomare, D., Ribaldi, F. et al. Societal and equity challenges for Brain Health Services. A user manual for Brain Health Services—part 6 of 6. Alz Res Therapy 13, 173 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-021-00885-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-021-00885-6

Keywords