- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Microstate connectivity alterations in patients with early Alzheimer’s disease
Alzheimer's Research & Therapy volume 7, Article number: 78 (2015)
Abstract
Introduction
Electroencephalography (EEG) microstates and brain network are altered in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and discussed as potential biomarkers for AD. Microstates correspond to defined states of brain activity, and their connectivity patterns may change accordingly. Little is known about alteration of connectivity in microstates, especially in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment with stable or improving cognition within 30 months (aMCI).
Methods
Thirty-five outpatients with aMCI or mild dementia (mean age 77 ± 7 years, 47 % male, Mini Mental State Examination score ≥24) had comprehensive neuropsychological and clinical examinations. Subjects with cognitive decline over 30 months were allocated to the AD group, subjects with stable or improving cognition to the MCI-stable group. Results of neuropsychological testing at baseline were summarized in six domain scores. Resting state EEG was recorded with 256 electrodes and analyzed using TAPEEG. Five microstates were defined and individual data fitted. After phase transformation, the phase lag index (PLI) was calculated for the five microstates in every subject. Networks were reduced to 22 nodes for statistical analysis.
Results
The domain score for verbal learning and memory and the microstate segmented PLI between the left centro-lateral and parieto-occipital regions in the theta band at baseline differentiated significantly between the groups. In the present sample, they separated in a logistic regression model with a 100 % positive predictive value, 60 % negative predictive value, 100 % specificity and 77 % sensitivity between AD and MCI-stable.
Conclusions
Combining neuropsychological and quantitative EEG test results allows differentiation between subjects with aMCI remaining stable and subjects with aMCI deteriorating over 30 months.
Introduction
In the United Kingdom cognitive decline affects approximately 18 % in the elderly [1], and early classification of the underlying pathology and prognosis is difficult. As personalized medicine may gain importance in the future, the distinction between patients with a prodromal syndrome of neurodegenerative dementia and patients with other reasons for cognitive impairment, such as as depressive and sleep disorders or neurovascular diseases, becomes relevant. For early identification of dementia, the term mild cognitive impairment (MCI) was defined as a potential prodromal syndrome without significant impairment in activities of daily living. This term was replaced with mild neurocognitive disorder in the revised Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, criteria [2]. The term is unspecific as the disorder can be caused by various pathologies [3], and a considerable number of patients with MCI remain stable or improve over time [4]. Diagnostic criteria for MCI due to Alzheimer’s disease were established [5, 6]. The rate of progression to AD in patients with MCI varies, depending on study design and definition of MCI. Maximally, 40 % of patients with amnestic (single or multiple domains) mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) at baseline progress to dementia within 2–3 years [7, 8]. However, novel treatment strategies require initiation of treatment at the earliest possible time [9]; therefore, the corroboration of the diagnosis and the identification of the cause of MCI are important.
Quantitative electroencephalography (qEEG) is increasingly being used to characterize cognitive impairment in different disorders [10–12]. However, it is unknown whether qEEG reliably identifies patients at an early stage of AD with clear progression of neuropsychological deficits and/or progression to dementia within the following years.
The aim of the present study was to find reliable qEEG biomarkers for the identification of patients with early cognitive deficits and at high risk of considerable cognitive decline and/or progression to AD dementia. The study included frequency analysis in signal space and a combined microstate and connectivity analysis. On the basis of the literature, patients with MCI progressing to AD are expected to show an increased connectivity in the theta band while the connectivity in the alpha and beta bands is decreased. Moreover, we hypothesize that alteration in connectivity in microstates correlates disparately with neuropsychological domain scores.
Methods
Patients
Thirty-five outpatients (Table 2) with either aMCI (n = 12) or mild AD (Mini Mental State Examination [MMSE] [13] score ≥24/30; n = 23) attending the Memory Clinic, University Center for Medicine of Aging Basel, Felix Platter-Hospital, Basel, Switzerland, participated in the study. aMCI was diagnosed according to the definition of Winblad et al. [3]. Probable AD was diagnosed according to the definition of McKhann et al. [14]. Exclusion criteria consisted of MMSE score <24/30, any significant diagnosis other than AD, and antiepileptic or antipsychotic drug treatment influencing electroencephalography (EEG) recordings. All patients taking benzodiazepines were either excluded or had their treatment stopped at least 48 h before EEG recording. Twenty-one patients had a clinical follow-up examination at about 30 months (mean observation time 30.1 ± 1.9 months, range 29–31 months), along with a reevaluation of their diagnoses. Among the patients with an initial diagnosis of probable AD, nine patients had a clinical examination and eight patients a standardized telephone visit at 30 months. For six patients, only a follow-up visit at 15 months was available. Yet, all 23 patients showed a decline in cognition over time. All 12 patients with an initial diagnosis of aMCI had a clinical follow-up visit at 30 months, and 9 patients had mild cognitive impairment with stable or improving cognition within 30 months (MCI-stable). Three patients with aMCI had deteriorated and were thus allocated for analysis to the AD group (n = 26).
For comparison purposes, a group of 26 cognitively healthy control (HC) subjects was frequency-matched to the AD group according to sex, age, and education (Table 2). Inclusion criteria for HC subjects were a subjective report of good health and neuropsychological examination results within normal limits (i.e., z-scores greater than or equal to −1.28). Exclusion criteria were a past and/or current diagnosis of any major brain disorder, alcoholism, psychiatric disorder, and general anesthesia within the previous 3 months. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (Ethikkomission beider Basel reference number 260/09). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Neuropsychological assessments
Raw scores derived from a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment battery were transformed into demographically (age, sex, and education level) adjusted z-scores [15]. Six domain scores were created according to Table 4 in the article by Beck et al. [16]. Briefly, these domains were assessed using the following tests: (1) for verbal attention, the digit span (forward and backward) from the German version of the Wechsler Memory Scale [17]; (2) for visual attention, the Corsi block-tapping test (forward and backward) from the German version of the Wechsler Memory Scale [17]; (3) for verbal learning memory, either the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (CERAD-NAB [18]; n = 13) or the German version of the California Verbal Learning Test [19] (n = 22); (4) for visual learning and memory, either the CERAD-NAB figures [18] or the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test [20]; (5) for verbal language production, the 15-item Boston Naming Test [18], animal fluency [21], and phonemic fluency (s-words) [22]; and (6) for executive motor ability, the Trail Making Test [23] and the Five-Point Test [24]. Executive visual ability could not be evaluated, because the Stroop effect test was not administered to the HC subjects.
EEG recording
EEG was recorded with a 256-channel EEG system (Geodesic EEG System 300, DC-amplifier, sampling rate 1000 Hz, high-pass filter 0.01 Hz, vertex reference, impedance ≤40 kΩ; Electrical Geodesics Inc. [EGI], Eugene, OR USA). Subjects were instructed to relax but to stay awake and to minimize eye and body movements. A continuous EEG with the subject’s eyes closed was recorded for 12 minutes. During data acquisition, a technician monitored a subset of electrodes online to check for vigilance and artifacts.
EEG preprocessing
A fully automated preprocessing procedure was carried out using Toolbox for Automated Processing of EEG (TAPEEG) v2.5 software (https://sites.google.com/site/tapeeg/) [25]. Briefly, segments of 25–200 seconds containing the least amount of artifacts and sleepiness were automatically selected. Data of 214 electrodes (excluding cheek and neck electrodes) were filtered (0.5–70 Hz, high-order least-squares filter), and bad channels were automatically detected by FASTER and FieldTrip algorithms [26, 27]. Using independent component analysis (EEGLAB [28]), components loading the electrocardiogram, line noise in single electrodes or single gross artifacts were excluded (at maximum 5 % of components). For epoch selection, the EEG was re-referenced to average reference, bad channels were interpolated using spherical splines [29], and a combined segment at least 180 seconds in length was created.
Spectral EEG analysis
Spectral analysis has been described elsewhere [30]. Twelve epochs of 4 seconds were automatically selected. Power spectra were calculated at each electrode (Thomson multitaper method), and at every electrode the median spectrum of the 12 epochs was determined. Spectral analysis was done on a regional level of spatial resolution based on 22 anatomically defined regions comprising 7 or 8 electrodes (n = 170, excluding electrodes in the midline and at the outer border) (see Additional file 1: Figure S1). From the median spectrum of each brain region, relative band power was calculated in five frequency bands (delta 1–4 Hz, theta 4–8 Hz, alpha1 8–10 Hz, alpha2 10–13 Hz, beta 13–30 Hz). Relative band power was defined as the absolute band power in a single frequency band divided by the band power 1–30 Hz. For statistical analysis, regional power was logit-transformed [31]. Peak and median frequency were determined at all parieto-occipital electrodes, and the median of the peak and median frequency were used for further analysis.
Microstate segmentation
The global field power (GFP) was calculated as the standard deviation of the data at each time point [32, 33]:
Only time points of local maxima of GFP were selected, and a k-means clustering with squared correlation as distance measure was used to obtain the most representative topographies. k-Means clustering was calculated for results with 2–20 clusters, and the optimal number of clusters was defined based on the Krzanowski-Lai criterion using an L-curve via an adaptive pruning algorithm [34], resulting in five different microstates as optimum. By fitting the individual data to the five template microstates using a temporal smoothing with a window size of 12 milliseconds [35], a vector was created for every subject, competitively labeling every time point to one of the five microstate classes.
Functional connectivity
Phase lag index (PLI) measures were first calculated using a standard approach. Data were filtered using a Butterworth filter to four predefined frequency bands (theta 4–8 Hz, alpha1 8–10 Hz, alpha2 10–13 Hz, beta 13–30 Hz). Phase estimation was archived using a Hilbert transformation. The phase difference distribution was obtained from a time series (t 1, …, t k) of phase differences (ΔΦ) between two signals, and the asymmetry of the phase difference distribution was calculated as described by Stam et al. [36]:
PLI was calculated using 12 epochs of 4 seconds and averaging the resulting matrix into 1 matrix per subject and frequency band. In the second approach, the microstate segmented phase lag index (msPLI) was calculated. For the calculation of msPLI, the Hilbert transformation was applied to the full-length EEG using a sliding window of 4 seconds with a 50 % Hanning window. For every microstate class, 4 stitched periods each of 4000 phase differences (4 seconds) were then extracted using the time frames indicated by the microstate label vector. The number of four epochs per microstate, subject, and frequency band was selected, as this minimal amount of epochs per microstate was available in almost all EEGs, given the recording time of EEG data. Like in the standard approach, the 20 resulting matrices (4 epochs × 5 microstates) were averaged per subject. For statistical analysis, electrodes were grouped into 22 regions of interest [25], comprising 11 regions per hemisphere, excluding electrodes in the midline, neck, and face.
Graph measures
Graph measures were calculated based on the full average msPLI weight matrix per subject in each frequency band (n = 214 nodes). To avoid arbitrary thresholds and unconnected nodes, weighted network analysis was employed in which each link was equivalent to the measured PLI of two interconnected nodes. Graph analysis results were calculated according to Table 1. The respective formulas were implemented in TAPEEG.
The weighted clustering coefficient C quantifies the intensities of the subgraphs of a node and is equivalent to the unweighted clustering coefficient normalized by the average intensities of triangles at the node if the weight matrix is symmetric and weights range between 0 and 1 [37]. The average overall C is the mean clustering coefficient (Cw), a global measure of functional segregation of the network [38, 39]. The weighted shortest path length L gives the average of the shortest distances of one node to each other node in the network, where shortest distance in the weighted case is defined as the smallest inverse of the sum of PLI values of connecting edges. The average overall L is the weighted average path length (Lw), a global measure of functional integration of the network [39, 40]. To make graph measures independent of network size and achieve better comparability between subjects, the measures were normalized [37]. Edge weights of an original network were randomly reshuffled, preserving network size but destroying network structure, and Cw and Lw were calculated for this random network. Using the average Cw and Lw of 50 surrogate random networks iterated five times in the denominator and Cw and Lw in the nominator, the normalized Cw or gamma and the normalized Lw or lambda were calculated. The degree diversity (Kw) represents the distribution of degrees in a network. The degree is the mean connectivity of a single node to all other nodes. A higher Kw stands for a network with only a few highly connected nodes, also called hubs [41]. The degree correlation expresses the amount of interconnection between nodes with similar degrees. This measure is related to the concept of a “rich club” [42], whereas nodes with higher degrees are preferentially interconnected. The distance matrix of a graph comprises all pairwise distances. Its maximum corresponds to the graph diameter, its minimum to the graph radius [41].
Statistics
Demographic characteristics were compared between groups using nonparametric tests. qEEG variables, including regional connectivities and results of neuropsychological tests, were compared between the three groups using analysis of variance (ANOVA). Subsequently, post hoc t tests between subgroups were applied. In case of regional EEG power analysis, permutation tests (ANOVA/t test with 10,000 permutations) were used [43]. A logistic regression analysis with backward elimination to classify groups was performed using the significant EEG measures from permutation tests and the neuropsychological test results at baseline as independent variables. Subsequently a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis was performed. Results with p values <0.05 were considered significant. Analyses were done using TAPEEG [25] and IBM SPSS® software (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA).
Results
Demographics
No significant differences between the three patient groups regarding age, education, or sex were found (Kruskal-Wallis test) (see Table 2). MMSE scores of patients with AD were significantly lower than those in the HC group (p < 0.01).
Neuropsychological assessments
A comparison of the cognitive dimensions scores is shown in Fig. 1. As expected, at baseline, patients with AD performed worse than HC subjects and the group of MCIstable individuals was in between.
Quantitative EEG
Frequency analysis
HC subjects and MCI-stable patients had significantly lower theta power and higher median frequency than patients with AD and tended to have higher alpha2 power. The results of regional analysis are shown in Fig. 2. Theta power differentiated most significantly between AD and MCI-stable (Table 3), classified with a sensitivity of 67 %, specificity of 85 %, positive predictive value of 88 %, and negative predictive value of 60 %. A higher relative theta power correlated with lower z-score for verbal learning and memory, visual learning and memory, executive motor ability, and verbal language production (Fig. 3).
Connectivity analysis
After correction for multiple comparisons, no significant differences were found in connectivity calculated without splitting by microstates. Using connectivities from microstates, significant differences in theta-band connectivity were detected (Fig. 4). The theta-band link between the left centrolateral and parieto-occipital regions differentiated most significantly between all three groups and between AD and MCI-stable with a sensitivity of 77 % and a specificity of 78 %. Connectivities were higher in the AD group than in the MCI-stable and HC groups. No significant differences were found for the connectivities in the alpha1, alpha2, and beta bands. Only a few links in the theta band correlated significantly with one of the six domain scores after correction for multiple comparisons. For results, see Fig. 5.
Graph analysis
Among the results of the graph analysis, only radius of theta connectomes differentiated the three groups. The radius was smaller in the AD group than in the MCI-stable and HC groups. Kw showed a trend toward higher values in the AD group than in the MCI-stable and HC groups (Fig. 6).
Logistic regression model
A binary logistic regression with the most significant results of the six domain scores at baseline and the frequency and connectivity analysis as well as with AD vs. HC as outcomes was calculated. After stepwise backward elimination, only the connectivity between left centrolateral and parieto-occipital regions (theta band) and verbal learning and memory remained in the model and were used for calculating a score. This score was used in a second logistic regression model in which we compared AD with MCI-stable. The ROC curve showed an area under the curve (AUC) of 0.90 (maximal Youden index sensitivity 77 %, specificity 100 %, positive predictive value 100 %, negative predictive value 60 %) (Fig. 7). Relative theta power was not part of the final model. Adding relative theta power as a third variable increased the AUC only minutely.
Discussion
A combination of qEEG with neuropsychological measures allows differentiation of patients with early AD from patients with aMCI who remain stable for 30 months with high sensitivity, specificity, and positive predictive value. For differentiation between beginning AD and other patients with MCI, the msPLI seemed to be superior to the PLI. This advantage is most probably explained by an increased signal-to-noise ratio. Microstates are believed to represent activity of different subnetworks of a global network [44, 45]. Using microstate segmentation, EEG periods reflecting the most active subnetworks were selectively included in the analysis, resulting in a more precise estimation of the averaged global network.
Increased msPLI connectivity between the left centrolateral and parieto-occipital regions in the theta band separated AD from MCI-stable with over 90 % positive predictive value and 54 % negative predictive value. Graph analysis in the theta band showed reduced radius and higher Kw in the AD group compared with the MCI-stable group and even more so compared with the HC group. The combined results of connectivity and graph analysis indicated a shift toward a more hierarchical network with a concentration in a few highly connected nodes, so-called hubs. Hubs are believed to decline disproportionately with disease progression in later stages of AD [46]. In contrast, the present study shows a relative increase of the degree of hubs in very early AD, possibly pointing toward compensatory functional overload leading to faster degradation later. A similar concept was also discussed in a recent article by Morabito et al. [47].
When theta connectivity and verbal learning and memory are combined in a model for predicting the cognitive deterioration of patients with aMCI or early AD, a clear separation between AD and MCI-stable is possible very early after the beginning of cognitive decline. It is not surprising that the domain score for verbal learning and memory remained in the model, as this cognitive domain is most affected in patients with AD [16]; however, the results of relative power analysis, well known to differentiate between groups [12, 30, 48], did not remain in the final model after stepwise backward elimination, as their contribution to the prediction of outcome was included mainly in the two other variables. In contrast, the result for theta connectivity seems to add independent information regarding the 30-month prognosis. The localization of the most affected connections in the parieto-occipital region may be explained by the localization of hubs in the parietal regions, which are known to be particularly affected in AD [46].
Limitations of the study include the small sample size and the low number of patients with mildly reduced yet stable cognition.
Conclusions
Integration of connectivity results and verbal learning and memory tests in a statistical model may allow for definition of cohorts of patients with MCI with an enhanced risk for AD, a stage at which clinical trials are most promising. The high positive predictive value of this model allows the definition of a patient cohort at great risk of fast cognitive deterioration at a time when they are only mildly affected.
Abbreviations
- AD:
-
Alzheimer’s disease
- aMCI:
-
amnestic mild cognitive impairment
- ANOVA:
-
analysis of variance
- AUC:
-
area under the curve
- CERAD-NAB:
-
Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Neuropsychological Assessment Battery
- Cw:
-
mean clustering coefficient
- EEG:
-
electroencephalography
- GFP:
-
global field power
- HC:
-
healthy control
- Kw:
-
degree diversity
- Lw:
-
weighted average path length
- MCI:
-
mild cognitive impairment
- MCI-stable:
-
amnestic mild cognitive impairment patients with stable or improving cognition within 30 months
- MMSE:
-
Mini Mental State Examination
- msPLI:
-
microstate segmented phase lag index
- PLI:
-
phase lag index
- qEEG:
-
quantitative electroencephalography
- ROC:
-
receiver operating characteristic
- Rw:
-
Pearson correlation of degrees of pairs of neighbors
- TAPEEG:
-
Toolbox for Automated Processing of EEG v2.5 software
- VLM:
-
verbal learning and memory
References
Rait G. Prevalence of cognitive impairment: results from the MRC trial of assessment and management of older people in the community. Age Ageing. 2005;34:242–8.
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC: Author; 2013.
Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L, Wahlund LO, et al. Mild cognitive impairment – beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. 2004;256:240–6.
Ritchie K, Artero S, Touchon J. Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment: a population-based validation study. Neurology. 2001;56:37–42.
Litvan I, Goldman JG, Tröster AI, Schmand BA, Weintraub D, Petersen RC, et al. Diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Movement Disorder Society Task Force guidelines. Mov Disord. 2012;27:349–56.
Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Feldman HH, Fox NC, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:270–9.
Duara R, Loewenstein DA, Greig MT, Potter E, Barker W, Raj A, et al. Pre-MCI and MCI: neuropsychological, clinical, and imaging features and progression rates. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;19:951–60.
Schmidtke K, Hermeneit S. High rate of conversion to Alzheimer’s disease in a cohort of amnestic MCI patients. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20:96–108.
Panza F, Frisardi V, Solfrizzi V, Imbimbo BP, Logroscino G, Santamato A, et al. Immunotherapy for Alzheimer’s disease: from anti-β-amyloid to tau-based immunization strategies. Immunotherapy. 2012;4:213–38.
Caviness JN, Hentz JG, Evidente VG, Driver-Dunckley E, Samanta J, Mahant P, et al. Both early and late cognitive dysfunction affects the electroencephalogram in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2007;13:348–54.
Fonseca LC, Tedrus GMAS, Letro GH, Bossoni AS. Dementia, mild cognitive impairment and quantitative EEG in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2009;40:168–72.
Roh JH, Park MH, Ko D, Park KW, Lee DH, Han C, et al. Region and frequency specific changes of spectral power in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122:2169–76.
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–98.
McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein MF, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939–44.
Berres M, Monsch AU, Bernasconi F, Thalmann B, Stahelin HB. Normal ranges of neuropsychological tests for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Stud Health Technol Inform. 2000;195–202.
Beck IR, Schmid NS, Berres M, Monsch AU. Establishing robust cognitive dimensions for characterization and differentiation of patients with Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, frontotemporal dementia and depression. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2014;29:624–34.
Härtig C, Markowitsch HJ, Neufeld H, Calabrese P, Deisinger K, Kessler J. Wechsler-Gedächtnistest: Revidierte Fassung (WMS-R). Bern, Switzerland: Huber; 2000. German adaptation of the Wechsler Memory Scale.
Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Hughes JP, van Belle G, Fillenbaum G, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1989;39:1159–65.
Delis D, Kramer J, Ober B, Kaplan E. The California Verbal Learning Test: administration and interpretation. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1987.
Osterrieth PA. Le test de copie d’une figure complexe; contribution à l’étude de la perception et de la mémoire [Test of copying a complex figure; contribution to the study of perception and memory.]. Arch Psychol. 1944;30:206–356.
Isaacs B, Kennie AT. The Set Test as an aid to the detection of dementia in old people. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci. 1973;123:467–70.
Thurstone LL, Thurstone TM. Chicago Test of Primary Mental Abilities. Chicago: Science Research Associates; 1948.
Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19:203–214.
Cattelani R, Dal Sasso F, Corsini D, Posteraro L. The Modified Five-Point Test: normative data for a sample of Italian healthy adults aged 16–60. Neurol Sci. 2011;32:595–601.
Hatz F, Hardmeier M, Bousleiman H, Rüegg S, Schindler C, Fuhr P. Reliability of fully automated versus visually controlled pre- and post-processing of resting-state EEG. Clin Neurophysiol. 2015;126:268–74.
Nolan H, Whelan R, Reilly RB. FASTER: Fully Automated Statistical Thresholding for EEG artifact Rejection. J Neurosci Methods. 2010;192:152–62.
Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen JM. FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011;2011:156869.
Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;134:9–21.
Perrin F, Pernier J, Bertrand O, Echallier JF. Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1989;72:184–7.
Hatz F, Benz N, Hardmeier M, Zimmermann R, Rueegg S, Schindler C, et al. Quantitative EEG and apolipoprotein E-genotype improve classification of patients with suspected Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;124:2146–52.
Gasser T, Bächer P, Möcks J. Transformations towards the normal distribution of broad band spectral parameters of the EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1982;53:119–24.
Murray MM, Brunet D, Michel CM. Topographic ERP analyses: a step-by-step tutorial review. Brain Topogr. 2008;20:249–64.
Wackermann J, Lehmann D, Michel CM, Strik WK. Adaptive segmentation of spontaneous EEG map series into spatially defined microstates. Int J Psychophysiol. 1993;14:269–83.
Krzanowski WJ, Lai YT. A criterion for determining the number of groups in a data set using sum-of-squares clustering. Biometrics. 1988;44:23–34.
Pascual-Marqui RD, Michel CM, Lehmann D. Segmentation of brain electrical activity into microstates: model estimation and validation. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 1995;42:658–65.
Stam CJ, Nolte G, Daffertshofer A. Phase lag index: assessment of functional connectivity from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28:1178–93.
Stam CJ, de Haan W, Daffertshofer A, Jones BF, Manshanden I, van Cappellen van Walsum AM, et al. Graph theoretical analysis of magnetoencephalographic functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2009;132:213–24.
Newman MEJ. Properties of highly clustered networks. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys. 2003;68:026121.
Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage. 2010;52:1059–69.
Latora V, Marchiori M. Efficient behavior of small-world networks. Phys Rev Lett. 2001;87:198701.
Sporns O, Honey CJ, Kötter R. Identification and classification of hubs in brain networks. PLoS One. 2007;2:e1049.
van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O. Rich-club organization of the human connectome. J Neurosci. 2011;31:15775–86.
Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;15:1–25.
Stam CJ, van Straaten ECW. The organization of physiological brain networks. Clin Neurophysiol. 2012;123:1067–87.
Van De Ville D, Britz J, Michel CM. EEG microstate sequences in healthy humans at rest reveal scale-free dynamics. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:18179–84.
Tijms BM, Wink AM, de Haan W, van der Flier WM, Stam CJ, Scheltens P, et al. Alzheimer’s disease: connecting findings from graph theoretical studies of brain networks. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34:2023–36.
Morabito FC, Campolo M, Labate D, Morabito G, Bonanno L, Bramanti A, et al. A longitudinal EEG study of Alzheimer’s disease progression based on a complex network approach. Int J Neural Syst. 2015;25:1550005.
Babiloni C, Cassetta E, Binetti G, Tombini M, Del Percio C, Ferreri F, et al. Resting EEG sources correlate with attentional span in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:3742–57.
Spoormaker VI, Schroter MS, Gleiser PM, Andrade KC, Dresler M, Wehrle R, et al. Development of a large-scale functional brain network during human non-rapid eye movement sleep. J Neurosci. 2010;30:11379–87.
Newman MEJ. Mixing patterns in networks. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys. 2003;67:026126.
Wang H, Hernandez JM, Van Mieghem P. Betweenness centrality in a weighted network. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys. 2008;77:046105.
Acknowledgments
We thank the participating subjects and caregivers. The financial support of the Synapsis/Parrotia Foundation, Swiss National Science Foundation (grants 33CM30-140338, 33CM30-124115 and 326030-128775/1), Novartis Research Foundation, Freiwillige Akademische Gesellschaft Basel and the Mach-Gaensslen Foundation is gratefully acknowledged.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Competing interests
MH: Novartis Research Foundation; UG: support from Mach-Gaensslen Foundation, Jacques and Gloria Gossweiler Foundation, Parkinson Schweiz, Synapsis Foundation, and Botnar Foundation; SR: Swiss National Science Foundation and Novartis Research Foundation; CS: Swiss National Science Foundation; AUM: Swiss National Science Foundation, Parkinson Schweiz, Gossweiler Foundation, Synapsis Foundation, Novartis, and Novartis Research Foundation; PF: research support from the Swiss National Science Foundation, Mach-Gaensslen-Foundation, Gossweiler Foundation, Parkinson Schweiz, Synapsis Foundation, Botnar Foundation, Freiwillige Akademische Gesellschaft Basel, Novartis Research Foundation, Novartis, Roche, and AbbVie. The other authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors’ contributions
FH recruited patients, processed the EEG data and drafted the manuscript. MH initiated and helped conduct the study and revised the manuscript. ME and NB recruited patients, were responsible for the neuropsychological examinations and revised the manuscript. CS participated in the design of the study, performed the statistical analysis and helped to revise the manuscript. PF, SR, UG and AUM conceived the study, participated in its design and coordination, and revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Additional file
Additional file 1: Figure S1.
Mapping of the 22 regions: 170 of 257 electrodes (HydroCel GSN; Electrical Geodesic Inc. [EGI]) were used to define the 22 regions. (TIF 2594 kb)
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
About this article
Cite this article
Hatz, F., Hardmeier, M., Benz, N. et al. Microstate connectivity alterations in patients with early Alzheimer’s disease. Alz Res Therapy 7, 78 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-015-0163-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-015-0163-9