Skip to main content

The criteria used to rule out mild cognitive impairment impact dementia incidence rates in subjective cognitive decline

Abstract

Background

The research criteria for subjective cognitive decline (SCD) exclude mild cognitive impairment (MCI), but do not stipulate the use of specific MCI criteria. This study compared different approaches to defining (i.e., excluding) MCI during the ascertainment of SCD, focusing on the impact on dementia incidence rates in SCD.

Methods

This cohort study utilized routine healthcare data collected in the Essex Memory Clinic from 1999 to 2023. Two different operationalizations of the SCD criteria were used to categorize the cohort into two SCD patient samples. One sample was based on local clinical practice – MCI was excluded according to the Winblad criteria (this sample was termed SCDWinblad). The other sample was created via the retrospective application of the Jak/Bondi criteria for the exclusion of MCI (termed SCDJak/Bondi). Only patients aged ≥ 55 years at baseline with ≥ 12 months follow-up were considered for inclusion. The initial clinical/demographic characteristics of the samples were compared. Rates of incident dementia were calculated for each sample, and unadjusted and Mantel-Haenszel-adjusted incidence rate ratios were calculated to compare dementia incidence between the SCD samples.

Results

The Essex Memory Clinic database included 2,233 patients in total. The SCD and study eligibility criteria were used to select SCDWinblad (n = 86) and SCDJak/Bondi (n = 185) samples from the database. Median follow-up (3 years) did not differ between the two samples. The SCDJak/Bondi sample was significantly older than the SCDWinblad at first assessment (median age: 74 versus 70 years) and had poorer scores on tests of global cognition, immediate and delayed verbal recall, and category fluency. Following adjustment for age, the dementia incidence rate ratio [95% confidence interval] was 3.7 [1.5 to 9.3], indicating a significantly greater rate of progression to dementia in SCDJak/Bondi.

Conclusions

This study highlights that the approach used to ascertain SCD has important implications for both SCD phenotypes and prognosis. This underscores the importance of how MCI is operationalized within SCD studies. More broadly, the findings add to a growing body of work indicating that objective cognition should not be overlooked in SCD, and offer a potential explanation for the heterogeneity across the SCD prognostic literature.

Background

Interest in the preclinical and prodromal stages of dementia continues to intensify within the research and medical communities. This trend is both motivated and typified by efforts to identify modifiable risk factors for dementia [1], as well as the recent emergence of disease-modifying immunotherapies for Alzheimer’s disease (AD) which target the mild dementia/mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stages [2].

Subjective cognitive difficulties in the absence of objective impairment on standardized tests are common in later life, affecting approximately 25% of the older population [3]. This phenomenon is clinically operationalized by the 2014 research criteria for subjective cognitive decline (SCD) [4]. Briefly, the criteria require both self-experienced decline in cognitive capacity, and normal performance on cognitive tests used to classify MCI and/or dementia [4].

Two meta-analyses [5, 6] and a multiple cohort analysis [7] found increased rates of incident dementia in people with SCD versus healthy controls. Nevertheless, individual prognosis is often uncertain [8], and there is significant heterogeneity in dementia incidence rates across the SCD literature [6]. A growing body of work has sought to identify individual- and study-level factors that may account for the variation in clinical outcomes in SCD [6, 7]. Wolfsgruber et al. [9] reported that, at a group level, SCD participants from the German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases Longitudinal Cognitive Impairment and Dementia Study (DELCODE) had minor neuropsychological deficits. The same group later reported that, at the individual level, neuropsychological deficits are associated with an increased risk of incident MCI in SCD [10]. These studies highlight that, whilst (by definition) people with SCD lack objective cognitive impairment, some have subtle objective deficits, with these individuals having a worse prognosis.

Writing for the SCD Initiative (SCD-I), Molinuevo et al. [11] reviewed the literature to characterize how the SCD criteria are implemented in research settings, and to make recommendations for the future. The authors noted that studies differed in the way they operationalized the ‘MCI exclusion criterion’ within the SCD criteria. Some defined MCI according to conventional (i.e., Petersen [12] or Winblad [13]) criteria, which require only a single impaired cognitive score to be fulfilled. Others utilized the Jak/Bondi MCI criteria, which require more than one impaired score (either across or within cognitive domains), in order to capture a more ‘reliably impaired’ MCI phenotype, with a greater risk of progression to dementia [14]. The criteria used to define MCI effectively demarcate the maximum degree of cognitive deficits that can be present and still fulfil SCD criteria [11]. The upshot of this is that how MCI is defined during the ascertainment of SCD may have implications for SCD phenotypes/prognosis.

Methods

Reporting guidelines

This study is reported according to the REporting of studies Conducted using Observational Routinely-collected health Data (RECORD) guidelines [15]; see supplementary Table S4 for the RECORD checklist.

Study design and setting

This study utilized routine healthcare data collected by the Essex Memory Clinic, a service operated by Essex Partnership University National Health Service Foundation Trust. The clinic provides in-depth assessment for patients presenting with cognitive symptoms. Clinical diagnosis is based on a consensus of professionals, including consultant old age psychiatrists and neuropsychologists. Assessment data (e.g., psychiatric/neuropsychological measures) are routinely recorded in a database to support healthcare provision and research.

Study population

The version of the database used for this study contained data collected from April 1999 to August 2023, and included 2,233 patients. Two subsamples of these patients were included in analyses, based on the fulfilment of study eligibility criteria; given the current focus (i.e., dementia incidence), both samples only included patients aged ≥ 55 years at their initial assessment, with at least one follow-up visit ≥ 12 months later. For both samples, patients had to fulfil the research criteria for SCD [4] following their initial assessment. The criteria require both self-experienced persistent decline in cognitive capacity, and normal demographically-adjusted performance on standardized cognitive tests used to classify MCI/dementia. Importantly, different criteria were used to diagnose (i.e., exclude) MCI from each sample. For the first sample, MCI was diagnosed (i.e., excluded) according to the Winblad MCI criteria [13], while the Jak/Bondi criteria [14] were used to exclude MCI from the second sample (see ‘Derivation of SCD samples’ below). In-keeping with the SCD criteria, both samples excluded patients with currently elevated depressive/anxiety symptoms, and/or a history of bipolar disorder, schizoaffective disorder, schizophrenia, or alcohol use disorder. Both samples also excluded individuals with a history of stroke, head injury with loss of consciousness, epilepsy or neurodegenerative disease.

Procedures

At patients’ initial assessment, a psychiatrist took a thorough medical, psychiatric and social history (including alcohol/tobacco use); and completed a neurological examination. Patients were also invited to undergo structural neuroimaging (MRI/CT) and blood screening for reversible causes of cognitive impairment. At both initial and follow-up assessments, a range of standardized psychiatric, neurological and medical measures, as well as a number of neuropsychological tests, were completed (see below); the medical history was also updated as applicable. Patients were asked to attend assessments with an informant where possible. For full details of clinical procedures, see Sadiq and colleagues [16].

Measures

Affective symptoms

As noted above, in line with the SCD criteria, individuals with currently elevated affective symptoms were excluded from this study. Excluding these individuals minimizes the confounding effects of affective symptoms on subjective cognition [17]. Different measures were used to capture affective symptoms within the data collection period. Depressive and anxiety symptoms were initially captured using the Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia (CSDD) [18], and the Rating Anxiety in Dementia (RAID) scale [19], respectively. These scales were subsequently replaced by the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), comprising anxiety (HADS-A) and depression (HADS-D) subscales [20]. Patients exceeding published clinical cut-offs (i.e., CSDD ≥ 12; RAID ≥ 11; HADS-A/D ≥ 11) [18,19,20] were excluded.

Objective cognition

Global cognitive status was measured using the Mini-mental state examination (MMSE; scoring range: 0–30) [21] and the Cambridge Cognitive Examination-Revised (CAMCOG-R; scoring range: 0–105). Scores < 81 on CAMCOG-R are suggestive of dementia [22]. Episodic memory was measured using immediate and delayed recall scores from the Logical Memory (LM) subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-third [23] or -fourth [24] edition. LM scores were entered on the database as age-adjusted percentile scores. Psychomotor speed and executive task switching were assessed using parts A and B of the Trail-Making Test (TMT-A and -B), respectively. Scores were recorded as completion time in seconds (note: TMT trials were discontinued for patients making ≥ 2 errors). Verbal fluency was assessed using category fluency (total animals in 60 s) and letter fluency for ‘F’, ‘A’, and ‘S’ (for each letter, total correct words in 60 s). For the application of Jak/Bondi MCI criteria (see ‘Derivation of SCD samples’), raw cognitive scores had to be converted to z-scores. Category fluency, letter fluency, TMT-A and TMT-B raw scores were z-scored using age- and education-specific norms [25, 26]. LM data were originally recorded as age-corrected percentile scores; in line with previous work [27], LM immediate/delayed scores ≤ 16th percentile (corresponding to ≥ 1 SD below norms) were classified as impaired. Until 2012, premorbid IQ was estimated using the National Adult Reading Test [28]; thereafter it was measured using the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading [29]. During routine diagnostic work-up (i.e., using Winblad criteria – see ‘Diagnosis’), the multidisciplinary team considered the patient’s amount of formal education, estimated premorbid IQ, and premorbid occupational functioning; for patients with higher premorbid functioning, a stricter/higher threshold for cognitive impairment was utilized (the converse was true for individuals with lower premorbid functioning).

Diagnosis

The criteria used to diagnose dementia varied over the data collection period, as criteria were superseded by updated versions. At present, dementia diagnosis is made according to the International Classification of Diseases (tenth revision) [30] in conjunction with dedicated criteria, including AD [31], vascular dementia (VaD) [32], dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) [33] and frontotemporal dementia (FTD) [34]. For analyses, patients progressing to AD/AD with cerebrovascular disease (CVD) were combined into a single category (i.e., AD ± CVD). In the clinic, the term ‘subjective cognitive impairment’ (SCI) (see [35]) is used, rather than SCD – reflecting that the SCD criteria [4] were not yet published at the clinic’s inception. The entity of SCI closely aligns with SCD, although SCI is more inclusive (e.g., psychiatric disorders are not exclusionary, except where these are adjudged to fully account for cognitive symptoms). In the clinic, the diagnosis of MCI is made according to Winblad criteria [13] (summarized in Table 1). Prior to the publication of the Winblad criteria (i.e., from 1999 to 2004), MCI was defined as one or more impaired cognitive scores without functional impairment (a conception broadly similar to that encapsulated by the Winblad criteria). Patients who receive a diagnosis of MCI or SCI are invited for reassessment at 1 or 2 years, respectively.

Table 1 Diagnostic criteria used in this study to operationalize the ‘MCI exclusion criterion’ within the SCD research criteria

Derivation of SCD samples

The derivation of the ‘first’ SCD sample essentially followed local clinical practice; only patients with SCI (see previous paragraph) who also fulfilled the SCD criteria [4] were included. For this sample, in line with routine practice, MCI was operationalized (excluded) according to Winblad criteria. For brevity, SCD diagnosed after ‘ruling out’ MCI according to Winblad criteria is hereafter denoted SCDWinblad (for consistency, the same convention is used to denote patients who fulfil Winblad criteria, that is, MCIWinblad).

A ‘second’ SCD sample was created for this study, by retrospectively applying the SCD criteria to the assessment data of all patients originally diagnosed with SCI/MCI (see ‘Diagnosis’). Importantly, for this sample, Jak/Bondi [14] (rather than Winblad) criteria were used to diagnose/exclude MCI. Only patients who fulfilled SCD criteria [4] but did not fulfil Jak/Bondi MCI criteria were included in the second sample. This sample is hereafter termed SCDJak/Bondi (patients fulfilling Jak/Bondi criteria for MCI are hereafter termed MCIJak/Bondi). Only patients originally categorized as SCI/MCI were considered for reclassification, because other diagnoses (e.g., dementia, psychiatric disorders) are excluded by SCD criteria. Importantly, the Winblad criteria require ‘self and/or informant report’ of cognitive dysfunction for the diagnosis of MCI. Thus, self-reported dysfunction (the defining feature of SCD) could not be assumed for patients with MCIWinblad. For these individuals, a case note review was undertaken; where self-reported difficulties could not be confirmed, patients were not eligible for reclassification as SCDJak/Bondi.

Jak/Bondi MCI criteria (see Table 1) require cognitive tests to be categorized into domains. For the present study, each domain subsumed two tests/scores; the domains (constituents) were: verbal fluency (category fluency and letter fluency); episodic memory (LM immediate and delayed recall); and (after Bondi et al. [14]) psychomotor speed/executive function (TMT-A and -B). Patients are classified as MCIJak/Bondi if either: (1) both scores within a domain fall more than 1 SD below the demographically-adjusted normative means; or (2) at least one score in all domains falls more than 1 SD below the normative mean. Missing values in these six measures (including discontinued TMT trials) were coded as impaired, because missingness was associated with poorer CAMCOG-R scores (see supplementary Methods), and because this approach effectively excluded patients with an unclear cognitive profile from the SCDJak/Bondi sample.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive and between-sample statistics were calculated for demographic, cognitive and follow-up characteristics. For cognitive variables used to operationalize Jak/Bondi criteria, z-scores are presented. For MMSE/CAMCOG-R, raw scores are reported. The proportion of missing data was also calculated and reported for each variable. To estimate the incidence rate of dementia, two new variables were calculated for each patient. The first was a binary event indicator (coded as 1/0 for patients who did/did not progress to dementia, respectively). A time-to-event (hereafter ‘follow-up’) variable was also calculated, capturing the time (in years) from initial assessment to diagnosis of dementia or end of follow-up (for patients who did not progress to dementia). For each sample, follow-up was summed across patients, corresponding to the total person-years at risk of dementia. The incidence rate of dementia was calculated for each sample by dividing the number of incident dementia cases by the total person-years at risk, and multiplying by 1,000. Incidence rates are thus interpretable as the number of new cases of dementia diagnosed amongst 1,000 patients with SCD each year [36].

Incidence rates of dementia were compared between samples via the incidence rate ratio, defined as the rate in SCDJak/Bondi divided by the rate in SCDWinblad. If between-sample differences were observed for any initial characteristic(s) (other than cognitive measures), this was treated as confounding and a Mantel-Haenszel adjusted incidence rate ratio was also calculated. For calculation, each sample is stratified according to the confounder, and the incidence rate is derived for each sample/stratum. Mantel-Haenszel methods are then used to combine the stratum-specific estimates [37]. The statistical significance of the unadjusted and Mantel-Haenszel adjusted incidence rate ratios is evaluated via a chi-squared test of the null hypothesis that the ratio is equal to 1.

Analyses were conducted in R 4.3.1 under RStudio 2023.12.1. The survival 3.5-7 package was used to calculate person-years at risk. The epi.2by2 function from the epiR 2.0.68 package was used to calculate the incidence rate for each sample, as well as the unadjusted and Mantel-Haenszel adjusted incidence rate ratios and 95% confidence intervals (CI). All statistical tests were two-sided and deemed significant at p < 0.05.

Results

Sample selection

See Fig. 1 for a flowchart outlining the derivation of the SCDWinblad (n = 86) and SCDJak/Bondi (n = 185) samples.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart detailing selection of SCDWinblad and SCDJak/Bondi samples. Abbreviations: SCD = Subjective cognitive decline; SCI = Subjective cognitive impairment; RAID = Rating Anxiety in Dementia scale; CSDD = Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia; HADS-A = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-Anxiety subscale; HADS-D = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-Depression subscale. aMissing date of birth, visit date and/or diagnosis data. bOn the database, the term SCI, rather than SCD, is used (see Methods). cPatients with missing data for measures of affective symptomatology (i.e., RAID/CSDD or HADS-A/HADS-D) or case notes were excluded, as it was not possible to determine their fulfilment of the SCD research criteria

Sample initial characteristics and follow-up durations

Descriptive and comparative statistics for the samples’ demographic, cognitive and follow-up characteristics are presented in Table 2. There were no significant between-sample differences for sex, years of education, ethnicity, nor year of first visit. However, the median [interquartile range; IQR] age (years) of the SCDJak/Bondi sample (74.4 [66.5 to 80.1]) was greater than that of the SCDWinblad sample (70.0 [61.7 to 74.8]; p < 0.01). For all z-scored cognitive tests, medians were > 0 for both groups, as would be expected for cognitively unimpaired samples. The samples significantly differed for CAMCOG-R, category fluency, LM immediate recall, and LM delayed recall (SCDWinblad > SCDJak/Bondi in each case); no significant differences were observed for MMSE, TMT-A/B, nor letter fluency.

Whilst the median [IQR] number of visits was greater in the SCDJak/Bondi versus SCDWinblad sample (3 [2 to 5], versus 3 [2 to 4]; p = 0.02), the length of follow-up (years) did not differ (3.2 [2.1 to 4.9], versus 3.4 [2.1 to 4.7]; p = 0.51).

Table 2 Sample demographic, cognitive and follow-up characteristics

Data cleaning and missing data

Prior to analysis, cognitive variables were checked for values falling outside the valid scoring range; aberrant values were corrected by referring back to case notes. Where this did not satisfactorily resolve queries, values were deleted. For the missingness proportions in each variable/sample, see supplementary Table S1. For both samples, missingness was ≤ 5% for all variables.

Overlap between samples

A total of 74 patients were common to both SCD samples (these individuals had SCD irrespective of the criteria used to exclude MCI). Twelve patients in the SCDWinblad sample were reclassified as MCI under Jak/Bondi criteria. The SCDJak/Bondi sample included the remaining 74 patients from the SCDWinblad sample, as well as 111 patients originally diagnosed with MCIWinblad.

Incidence rates of dementia

Of the 86 patients in the SCDWinblad sample, 5 (6%) progressed to dementia (3 to AD ± CVD, 1 to DLB, and 1 to VaD; see Table 3) during 335 person-years of observation. The incidence rate [95% CI] of dementia was 14.9 [4.9 to 34.9] per 1,000 person-years.

Table 3 Diagnostic outcomes at follow-up

Of the 185 patients in the SCDJak/Bondi sample, 52 (28%) progressed to dementia (41 to AD ± CVD, 5 to DLB, 3 to VaD, 2 to FTD, and 1 to unspecified dementia) during 689 person-years of observation. The incidence rate [95% CI] of dementia was 75.5 [56.4 to 98.9] per 1,000 person-years.

Incidence rate ratio

The unadjusted incidence rate ratio [95% CI] was 5.1 [2.0 to 16.2], indicating that the incidence rate of dementia was greater (p < 0.01) in the SCDJak/Bondi (versus SCDWinblad) sample. However, the SCDJak/Bondi sample was on average 4.4 years older than the SCDWinblad sample (median 74.4 versus 70.0 years; p < 0.01). This difference was a confound, as dementia risk increases with age [38]. To derive an adjusted estimate, both samples were age-stratified, and incidence rate ratios calculated for each pair of strata (see Table 4); estimates were then combined using Mantel-Haenszel methods. Following Mantel-Haenszel adjustment for age, the overall ratio was nominally attenuated (3.7 [1.5 to 9.3]), but continued to indicate a greater dementia incidence rate in SCDJak/Bondi (p < 0.01).

Table 4 Overall and age strata-specific incidence rate ratios

Proportion progressing to MCI

The proportion of patients with a ‘final’ diagnosis of MCI was greater in the SCDWinblad versus SCDJak/Bondi sample (30% versus 11%; p < 0.01).

Discussion

This study harnessed routinely-collected memory clinic data to define two SCD samples according to different operationalizations of the Jessen et al. research criteria [4], and compared the incidence rate of dementia between them. The main finding was that the age-corrected incidence rate of dementia was over threefold greater in SCD patients when the Jak/Bondi (versus Winblad) criteria were used to exclude MCI.

Interestingly, the SCDJak/Bondi sample (n = 185) was larger than the SCDWinblad (n = 86) sample. The retrospective application of the Jak/Bondi criteria resulted in 111 patients originally diagnosed with MCI (according to Winblad criteria) being reclassified as SCD. Whilst some patients were reclassified from SCDWinblad to MCIJak/Bondi, these were fewer in number (n = 12). Interestingly, none of these 12 individuals progressed to dementia during follow-up. Whilst the increased dementia incidence rate in SCDJak/Bondi thus appears to be driven by the inclusion of patients formerly classified as MCIWinblad, it may also reflect that 12 patients who did not progress to dementia were included in the SCDWinblad but not the SCDJak/Bondi sample. Importantly, the SCDJak/Bondi sample was older than the SCDWinblad sample. Dementia risk increases with age [38] – it is thus likely that, by broadening the definition of SCD to include individuals with worse cognition, a greater number of older adults with incipient cognitive decline were captured.

The SCDJak/Bondi (versus SCDWinblad) sample had lower baseline scores on the CAMCOG-R, category fluency, and both immediate and delayed LM recall. A previous study of cognitively unimpaired participants found that scores on the 5-item preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite (PACC5) – which measures global cognition, category fluency, verbal memory and executive function – are inversely associated with cerebral amyloid-β load [39]. Moreover, an earlier study from the Essex Memory Clinic found that poorer global cognition and delayed verbal recall predicted incident AD in a combined SCD/MCI sample [40]. The present finding that, versus SCDWinblad, SCDJak/Bondi is characterized by poorer global cognition, category fluency and verbal recall, as well as an increased risk of dementia (predominantly due to AD) is thus broadly in-keeping with prior work linking specific cognitive profiles to early AD.

We observed dementia incidence rates of 15 and 76 per 1,000 person-years in SCDWinblad and SCDJak/Bondi, respectively. Slot et al. [7] leveraged six memory clinic cohorts to estimate the incidence rate of dementia in 1,530 patients with SCD (mean ± SD age: 67 ± 9 years); five of the cohorts excluded MCI on the basis of a single impaired score, while the remaining cohort excluded MCI via Jak/Bondi criteria. Pooling across cohorts, Slot et al. reported a dementia incidence rate of 20 per 1,000 person-years. In a study using National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) data, Ward et al. [41] calculated that the incidence rate of dementia was 134 per 1,000 person-years in 3,428 patients with MCI (age: 76 ± 7 years) diagnosed on the basis of a single impaired score. The incidence rate in the current SCDWinblad sample thus appears broadly comparable to that observed in the multicenter study by Slot et al. [7] (which largely employed a similar approach to excluding MCI). The incidence rate in the SCDJak/Bondi sample is intermediate between Slot et al. and the MCI sample from NACC [41]. Given the SCDJak/Bondi sample comprised 74 patients originally classified as SCDWinblad, plus 111 reclassified from MCIWinblad, this intermediate incidence rate appears plausible. Moreover, a previous study by Rhodius-Meester et al. [42] reported prognostic data for three SCD samples, one of which was ascertained by the German Dementia Competence Network (DCN; n = 269). Importantly, the DCN use Jak/Bondi criteria for MCI [43]; their SCD operationalization thus appears analogous to SCDJak/Bondi. Rhodius-Meester et al. [42] did not report dementia incidence rates, but they did include sufficient data to calculate these – we calculated a rate of 54 per 1,000 years for the DCN sample. This rate appears lower than that observed for SCDJak/Bondi in the current study (76 per 1,000 years); however, the current patients were older than the DCN sample (age: 73 ± 9 versus 66 ± 8 years). Whilst the incidence rate of dementia in SCDJak/Bondi may thus be higher than expected for an SCD sample, it remains broadly congruent with findings from other cohorts.

Whilst fewer patients developed dementia in the SCDWinblad (versus SCDJak/Bondi) sample, a greater proportion had MCI at their last available assessment (30% versus 11%). Whilst this may caution against conceptualizing SCDWinblad as a reliably ‘benign’ or ‘stable’ phenotype, the between-sample difference in MCI incidence does not affect the overall conclusions of this study; the majority of SCDJak/Bondi patients who progressed to dementia would likely have transitioned through the MCI stage (not reflected in the ‘final’ proportions with MCI reported above). Moreover, MCI does not always progress to dementia [44], and can revert (i.e., improve) in around 8% of cases in clinical settings [45]. Thus, whilst a greater proportion of patients with SCDWinblad progressed to MCI, overall this phenotype had a markedly better prognosis (i.e., a lower dementia incidence rate) versus SCDJak/Bondi. One question not answerable using the current design is whether there is a difference in the rate of cognitive decline between SCDWinblad and SCDJak/Bondi; this could be a fruitful line of investigation for future research.

Recall that Jak/Bondi criteria employ a conservative cut-off to classify cognitive scores as impaired (-1 SD below the normative mean), but that more than one impaired score is required for MCI (see Methods/Table 1). Bondi et al. [14] retrospectively applied the Jak/Bondi MCI criteria to a sample of Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative participants, including 846 patients with MCIWinblad, and 304 cognitively unimpaired individuals (without SCD). Following reclassification, 401 participants had MCIJak/Bondi, while 749 were cognitively unimpaired. Compared to the original MCIWinblad sample, MCIJak/Bondi was characterized by more consistent cognitive impairment; a greater proportion of APOE ε4 carriers; more AD-like CSF profiles; and a greater dementia incidence rate. In the present study, the application of Jak/Bondi criteria similarly classified a smaller number of individuals as MCI (here resulting in a greater number with SCD). In summary, Jak/Bondi criteria take a more conservative (versus Winblad) neuropsychological approach to defining MCI, resulting in fewer, more impaired ‘cases’ of MCI, with a greater risk of dementia (i.e., more specific for predicting progression). Conversely, Jak/Bondi criteria may be more prone to miss subtle cognitive deficits (i.e., less sensitive for predicting progression [46]). The use of Jak/Bondi criteria to rule out MCI thus appears to capture an SCD phenotype with worse objective cognition/prognosis.

Whilst Jak/Bondi and Winblad criteria take different approaches to operationalizing MCI, they are not mutually exclusive, resulting in a degree of ‘overlap’ between the current SCD samples. An alternative would be to categorize patients into three non-overlapping groups: those with SCD irrespective of MCI criteria (SCDWinblad/SCDJak/Bondi), and those with SCD under one criteria but MCI under the other (i.e., SCDWinblad/MCIJak/Bondi and MCIWinblad/SCDJak/Bondi). This approach may improve prognostic predictions. Unfortunately, this study lacked statistical power to investigate this empirically. In any case, this may be a primarily research-oriented question, as most clinical settings only utilize one type of MCI criteria.

The prognostic implications of different approaches to excluding MCI during the ascertainment of SCD have previously been discussed [11, 43], but empirical data have been lacking. Nevertheless, there is increasing recognition that SCD may not be synonymous with entirely ‘normal’ objective cognition, and that minor neuropsychological deficits have prognostic value in SCD. Using DELCODE data, Wolfsgruber et al. [9] demonstrated that, at the group level, patients with SCD have minor neuropsychological deficits (approximately 0.25–0.5 SD in magnitude) versus controls. The same group recently showed that SCD patients with (versus without) minor neuropsychological deficits had a faster cognitive decline and increased risk of MCI [10]. The DELCODE investigators operationalized MCI as a deficit of at least 1.5 SD on any test – an approach comparable to that used for the SCDWinblad sample in the current study. In spite that the SCD sample in DELCODE most closely aligns with the sample with better cognition/prognosis in the current study, those DELCODE SCD participants with subtle cognitive deficits continued to have a worse prognosis. In summary, despite that objective cognition is unimpaired in SCD, variation in scores (comfortably within the normal range) is linked to prognosis, both at the between-individual and between-sample levels. Whilst this suggests that the neuropsychological cut-offs employed by MCI criteria miss subtle – yet prognostically meaningful – cognitive deficits, more sensitive/thorough tests may be required to capture them, which are not available in all clinical settings.

The finding that excluding MCI via more stringent criteria yielded an SCD sample with worse cognition/prognosis is, arguably, unsurprising. Nevertheless, this finding remains important; there is significant heterogeneity in dementia incidence rates across the SCD literature [5, 47], and recent critiques have questioned the prognostic value of an SCD ‘diagnosis’ [48, 49]. Attempts to explain the heterogeneous dementia incidence rates in SCD have generally been unsuccessful. A recent meta-analysis evaluated numerous potential moderators of dementia incidence in SCD studies (including how SCD is defined, demographic/genetic factors, recruitment source, and follow-up duration) [6], but no significant moderators were identified (note: statistical power may have been lacking). Interestingly, neither the type of criteria used to exclude MCI, nor objective cognition more generally, were explored as candidate moderators. Whilst, in clinical practice, prognostic evaluation is primarily informed by individual patient characteristics, the present work suggests that the particular approach used to define MCI in a given clinical setting has important prognostic implications for individuals with SCD [4].

Reviewing the operationalization of SCD in research settings for the SCD-I, Molinuevo et al. [11] encouraged greater harmonization of SCD characterization across studies, to facilitate comparisons and evidence synthesis. Nevertheless, the authors acknowledged that variation in how SCD is defined has advanced scientific understanding in the field. Indeed, the authors did not recommend specific MCI criteria for SCD studies, because: the SCD/MCI boundary may not be clearcut, neuropsychological batteries vary across settings, and the choice of ‘liberal’ or ‘conservative’ MCI criteria depends on research questions. We echo the recommendations of the SCD-I: the optimum approach taken to exclude MCI in future SCD studies will likely depend on the research question. For example, in clinical trials targeting AD, the Jak/Bondi criteria may be optimal for excluding MCI (in order to ‘enrich’ the sample for preclinical AD and maximize statistical power [50]).

Strengths

The current work has a number of strengths. The present use of routinely-collected clinical data is no doubt a strength, given dementia risk is elevated in clinical, but not community-dwelling SCD populations [7]. This also increases the generalizability of the work to settings which assess and provide prognosis to individuals with cognitive symptoms. Moreover, all patients in the study underwent a thorough medical, neurological and psychiatric evaluation, and were assessed using a range of neuropsychological tests. This thorough characterization enabled the derivation of two samples fulfilling SCD criteria, differing only in the neuropsychological criteria used to operationalize (i.e., exclude) MCI. Furthermore, patients were followed-up for an average of 3 years, enabling dementia incidence rates to be estimated. The diagnosis of dementia (including subtype) was made by consensus, which improves diagnostic accuracy compared to individual clinicians [51]. Lastly, the calculation of the incidence rate ratio made use of Mantel-Haenszel methods which enabled adjustment for the confounding effect of age.

Limitations

This study also has limitations. To derive adequate samples of SCD patients, we used all available data (collected over three decades). Jutten et al. [52] discuss how ‘average performance’ on cognitive tests can drift over time, rendering norms outdated. Our reclassification of patients according to ‘fixed’ cognitive norms may thus have delineated subtly different SCD phenotypes at different times (although the temporal distribution of assessments appeared comparable between samples – see Table 2). This limitation partly reflects that the data were collected during routine healthcare at a single center [15]. Efforts to replicate the current findings in data collected within tighter timeframes (perhaps using prospective multicenter designs) are thus welcomed. The administration of TMT in this study differed from the typical procedure – here, patients were discontinued at two errors, whereas in other settings, assessors correct all errors without discontinuing patients (though the potential impact of this on results appears limited; see supplementary Discussion note 1). The MCI criteria utilized in this study vary in the precision with which they define cognitive impairment. The Winblad criteria require ‘impairment on objective cognitive tasks’, while the Jak/Bondi criteria feature specific cutoffs according to population norms. Both samples had a very high proportion of white individuals; whilst this is in-keeping with the characteristics of the population served by the memory clinic (see supplementary Discussion note 2), further research in samples with better representation of other ethnic groups will be required to assess the extent to which the current findings generalize in other populations. Further, the normative data used to calculate z-scores for each domain did not adjust for identical demographic factors; for verbal fluency and psychomotor speed/executive function, age- and education-adjusted norms were used, whereas the available norms for episodic memory only adjusted for age. Nor did we have access to ethnically and culturally appropriate test norms; a previous study reported that using ‘combined’ ethnicity norms can result in misclassifying scores for some groups [53]. In the interests of standardization and replicability, future SCD investigators are encouraged to characterize samples using ethnically and educationally appropriate norms, and to employ MCI criteria with precise cutoffs. Furthermore, the available tests permitted only three cognitive domains to be operationalized for Jak/Bondi criteria; given one of the ‘routes’ to diagnosis of MCIJak/Bondi is one impaired score within each assessed domain, this could have led to overdiagnosis of MCI, though Bondi et al. [14] also utilized three cognitive domains in their validation study. Considering the specific measures used to characterize cognitive domains, the tests used by Bondi et al. [14] for the language and episodic memory domains appeared less correlated than in the present study (see supplementary Discussion note 3). This could have resulted in a degree of diagnostic misclassification in the current study; future investigators are encouraged to utilize a greater number/range of cognitive domains, as well as sufficiently independent constituent tests, in order to mitigate this possibility. The defining characteristic of SCD is a self-experienced decline in cognitive capacity [4]. A retrospective case note review was required to ascertain the presence/absence of this feature for some patients, and this could not be determined in a small number, resulting in their exclusion. More generally, we did not have access to a standardized measure of subjective cognition (e.g., the SCD interview [54]), which precluded a comparison of subjective cognitive profiles between samples. This could be a fruitful topic for future investigations. Lastly, there is increasing interest in the role of biomarkers for prognostication in SCD [55]. Unfortunately, a lack of comprehensive biomarker data prevented the characterization of SCD according to (e.g.) the ATN framework [56]; we recommend that future projects include biomarker data where possible.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this study harnessed routine healthcare data to evaluate the prognostic implications of alternative operationalizations of the SCD research criteria. Compared to SCDWinblad, the SCDJak/Bondi sample was larger, older and had worse initial objective cognition. Following adjustment for age, the rate of incident dementia was over threefold greater in SCDJak/Bondi versus SCDWinblad. The present work adds to the literature highlighting the prognostic utility of objective cognition in individuals with SCD. This may partially account for the heterogeneity in the SCD prognostic literature [6], and facilitate wider efforts to better characterize and standardize approaches to defining SCD [11].

Data availability

The data underpinning this study were acquired during routine healthcare, and the patients did not consent to external data sharing. Moreover, the HRA has not approved external data sharing.

Abbreviations

AD:

Alzheimer’s disease

AD ± CVD:

Alzheimer’s disease with or without cerebrovascular disease

ATN:

Amyloid, Tau and Neurodegeneration

CAMCOG-R:

Cambridge Cognitive Examination-Revised

CI:

Confidence interval

CODEC:

Predictors of COgnitive DECline in attenders of memory clinic

CSDD:

Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia

DELCODE:

German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases Longitudinal Cognitive Impairment and Dementia Study

DLB:

Dementia with Lewy bodies

FTD:

Frontotemporal dementia

HADS:

Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale

HADS-A:

Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-anxiety subscale

HADS-D:

Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-depression subscale

HRA:

Health Research Authority

IQR:

Interquartile range

LM:

Logical Memory

MCI:

Mild cognitive impairment

MMSE:

Mini-mental state examination

NACC:

National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center

PACC5:

5-item preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite

RAID:

Rating Anxiety in Dementia scale

RECORD:

REporting of studies Conducted using Observational Routinely-collected health Data

SCD:

Subjective cognitive decline

SCD-I:

Subjective cognitive decline-Initiative

SCI:

Subjective cognitive impairment

TMT:

Trail-Making Test

TMT-A:

Trail-Making Test part A

TMT-B:

Trail-Making Test part B

VaD:

Vascular dementia

References

  1. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396(10248):413–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Self WK, Holtzman DM. Emerging diagnostics and therapeutics for Alzheimer disease. Nat Med. 2023;29(9):2187–99.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Rohr S, Pabst A, Riedel-Heller SG, Jessen F, Turana Y, Handajani YS, et al. Estimating prevalence of subjective cognitive decline in and across international cohort studies of aging: a COSMIC study. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2020;12(1):167.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Jessen F, Amariglio RE, van Boxtel M, Breteler M, Ceccaldi M, Chetelat G, et al. A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(6):844–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Mitchell AJ, Beaumont H, Ferguson D, Yadegarfar M, Stubbs B. Risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older people with subjective memory complaints: meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2014;130(6):439–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Pike KE, Cavuoto MG, Li L, Wright BJ, Kinsella GJ. Subjective cognitive decline: level of risk for future dementia and mild cognitive impairment, a Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal studies. Neuropsychol Rev. 2022;32(4):703–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Slot RER, Sikkes SAM, Berkhof J, Brodaty H, Buckley R, Cavedo E, et al. Subjective cognitive decline and rates of incident Alzheimer’s disease and non-alzheimer’s disease dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15(3):465–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Jessen F, Amariglio RE, van der Buckley RF, Han Y, Molinuevo JL, et al. The characterisation of subjective cognitive decline. Lancet Neurol. 2020;19(3):271–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Wolfsgruber S, Kleineidam L, Guski J, Polcher A, Frommann I, Roeske S, et al. Minor neuropsychological deficits in patients with subjective cognitive decline. Neurology. 2020;95(9):e1134–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Stark M, Wolfsgruber S, Kleineidam L, Frommann I, Altenstein S, Bartels C et al. Relevance of minor neuropsychological deficits in patients with subjective cognitive decline. Neurology. 2023.

  11. Molinuevo JL, Rabin LA, Amariglio R, Buckley R, Dubois B, Ellis KA, et al. Implementation of subjective cognitive decline criteria in research studies. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13(3):296–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Petersen RC, Morris JC. Mild cognitive impairment as a clinical entity and treatment target. Arch Neurol. 2005;62(7):1160–3. discussion 7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L, Wahlund LO, et al. Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on mild cognitive impairment. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):240–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Bondi MW, Edmonds EC, Jak AJ, Clark LR, Delano-Wood L, McDonald CR, et al. Neuropsychological criteria for mild cognitive impairment improves diagnostic precision, biomarker associations, and progression rates. J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;42(1):275–89.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Benchimol EI, Smeeth L, Guttmann A, Harron K, Moher D, Petersen I, et al. The REporting of studies conducted using Observational routinely-collected health data (RECORD) statement. PLoS Med. 2015;12(10):e1001885.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Sadiq D, Whitfield T, Lee L, Stevens T, Costafreda S, Walker Z. Prodromal Dementia with Lewy Bodies and Prodromal Alzheimer’s Disease: a comparison of the cognitive and clinical profiles. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;58(2):463–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Hill NL, Mogle J, Wion R, Munoz E, DePasquale N, Yevchak AM, et al. Subjective cognitive impairment and affective symptoms: a systematic review. Gerontologist. 2016;56(6):e109–27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Alexopoulos GS, Abrams RC, Young RC, Shamoian CA. Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia. Biol Psychiatry. 1988;23(3):271–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Shankar KK, Walker M, Frost D, Orrell MW. The development of a valid and reliable scale for rating anxiety in dementia (RAID). Aging Ment Health. 1999;3(1):39–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983;67(6):361–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Roth M, Huppert FA, Tym E, Mountjoy CQ. CAMDEX-R: The Revised Cambridge Examination for Mental Disorders of the Elderly. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1999 1999.

  23. Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale–Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory scale–Fourth Edition (UK). London: Pearson Assessment; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;14(2):167–77.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Tombaugh TN. Trail making test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19(2):203–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Brooks BL, Iverson GL, Holdnack JA, Feldman HH. Potential for misclassification of mild cognitive impairment: a study of memory scores on the Wechsler Memory Scale-III in healthy older adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2008;14(3):463–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Venegas J, Clark E. National Adult Reading Test. In: Kreutzer JS, DeLuca J, Caplan B, editors. Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2011. p. 1705.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  29. Venegas J, Clark E. Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. In: Kreutzer JS, DeLuca J, Caplan B, editors. Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2011. pp. 2693–4.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  30. World Health Organization. Chapter V: Organic, including symptomatic, mental disorders (F00-F09). In International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (11th ed.) 2019 [ https://icd.who.int/browse10/2019/en#/F00-F09.

  31. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr., Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):263–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. O’Brien JT, Thomas A. Vascular dementia. Lancet. 2015;386(10004):1698–706.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. McKeith IG, Boeve BF, Dickson DW, Halliday G, Taylor JP, Weintraub D, et al. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: fourth consensus report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology. 2017;89(1):88–100.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman D, Mendez MF, Kramer JH, Neuhaus J, et al. Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2011;134(Pt 9):2456–77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Rodda JE, Dannhauser TM, Cutinha DJ, Shergill SS, Walker Z. Subjective cognitive impairment: increased prefrontal cortex activation compared to controls during an encoding task. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;24(8):865–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Kirkwood BR, Sterne JAC. Longitudinal studies, rates and the Poisson distribution. Essential Medical Statistics: Wiley-Blackwell; 2003. pp. 227–39.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Kirkwood BR, Sterne JAC. Comparing rates. Essential Medical Statistics: Wiley-Blackwell; 2003. pp. 240–8.

    Google Scholar 

  38. van der Flier WM, Scheltens P. Epidemiology and risk factors of dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76(Suppl 5):v2–7.

  39. Papp KV, Rentz DM, Orlovsky I, Sperling RA, Mormino EC. Optimizing the preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite with semantic processing: The PACC5. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2017;3(4):668–77.

  40. Macdougall A, Whitfield T, Needham K, Schott JM, Frost C, Walker Z. Predicting progression to Alzheimer’s disease dementia using cognitive measures. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2024;39(2):e6067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Ward DD, Wallace LMK, Rockwood K. Frailty and risk of dementia in mild cognitive impairment subtypes. Ann Neurol. 2021;89(6):1221–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Rhodius-Meester HFM, Liedes H, Koikkalainen J, Wolfsgruber S, Coll-Padros N, Kornhuber J, et al. Computer-assisted prediction of clinical progression in the earliest stages of AD. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2018;10:726–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Wolfsgruber S, Polcher A, Koppara A, Kleineidam L, Frolich L, Peters O, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers and clinical progression in patients with subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;58(3):939–50.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Mitchell AJ, Shiri-Feshki M. Rate of progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia–meta-analysis of 41 robust inception cohort studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;119(4):252–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Canevelli M, Grande G, Lacorte E, Quarchioni E, Cesari M, Mariani C, et al. Spontaneous reversion of mild cognitive impairment to normal cognition: a systematic review of literature and Meta-analysis. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2016;17(10):943–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Oltra-Cucarella J, Sanchez-SanSegundo M, Lipnicki DM, Sachdev PS, Crawford JD, Perez-Vicente JA, et al. Using base rate of low scores to identify progression from Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment to Alzheimer’s Disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018;66(7):1360–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Abdulrab K, Heun R. Subjective memory impairment. A review of its definitions indicates the need for a comprehensive set of standardised and validated criteria. Eur Psychiatry. 2008;23(5):321–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. McWhirter L, Ritchie C, Stone J, Carson A. Functional cognitive disorders: a systematic review. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7(2):191–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Howard R. Subjective cognitive decline: what is it good for? Lancet Neurol. 2020;19(3):203–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Ballard C, Atri A, Boneva N, Cummings JL, Frolich L, Molinuevo JL, et al. Enrichment factors for clinical trials in mild-to-moderate Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2019;5:164–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Thorlacius-Ussing G, Bruun M, Gjerum L, Frederiksen KS, van der Rhodius-Meester HFM et al. Comparing a Single Clinician Versus a Multidisciplinary Consensus Conference Approach for Dementia Diagnostics. J Alzheimers Dis. 2021;83(2):741 – 51.

  52. Jutten RJ, Thompson L, Sikkes SAM, Maruff P, Molinuevo JL, Zetterberg H, et al. A neuropsychological perspective on defining cognitive impairment in the clinical study of Alzheimer’s disease: towards a more continuous Approach. J Alzheimers Dis. 2022;86(2):511–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Werry AE, Daniel M, Bergstrom B. Group differences in normal neuropsychological test performance for older non-hispanic White and Black/African American adults. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(8):1089–100.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Miebach L, Wolfsgruber S, Polcher A, Peters O, Menne F, Luther K, et al. Which features of subjective cognitive decline are related to amyloid pathology? Findings from the DELCODE study. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2019;11(1):66.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Rostamzadeh A, Bohr L, Wagner M, Baethge C, Jessen F. Progression of Subjective Cognitive decline to MCI or Dementia in relation to biomarkers for Alzheimer Disease: a Meta-analysis. Neurology. 2022;99(17):e1866–74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Jack CR Jr., Bennett DA, Blennow K, Carrillo MC, Feldman HH, Frisoni GB, et al. A/T/N: an unbiased descriptive classification scheme for Alzheimer disease biomarkers. Neurology. 2016;87(5):539–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the patients whose data underpinned this study.

Funding

This work was funded by The Dunhill Medical Trust [RTF1806\45]. This funding source had no role in the design, execution, analysis, or interpretation of this study; nor any role in the decision to submit to the journal.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Substantial contributions to the conception or design of the work (TW, NLM, ZW). Substantial contributions to the acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of the data (TW, LC, DRu, DRa, NLM, ZW). Drafting the work or reviewing it critically for important intellectual content (TW, LC, RM, DRu, DRa, NLM, ZW). Final approval of the version to be published (TW, LC, RM, DRu, DRa, NLM, ZW).

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tim Whitfield.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study is part of the ‘Predictors of COgnitive DECline in attenders of memory clinic’ (CODEC) project, which is approved by the UK Health Research Authority (HRA; ID 227614). Separate research ethics committee review was not required by the HRA, because this project utilizes anonymized data. Nevertheless, all patients provided written consent for their data to be utilized for research projects.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Whitfield, T., Chouliaras, L., Morrell, R. et al. The criteria used to rule out mild cognitive impairment impact dementia incidence rates in subjective cognitive decline. Alz Res Therapy 16, 142 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-024-01516-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-024-01516-6

Keywords