Skip to main content

Who am I with my Lewy bodies? The insula as a core region of the self-concept networks

Abstract

Background

Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) is characterized by insular atrophy, which occurs at the early stage of the disease. Damage to the insula has been associated with disorders reflecting impairments of the most fundamental components of the self, such as anosognosia, which is a frequently reported symptom in patients with Lewy bodies (LB). The purpose of this study was to investigate modifications of the self-concept (SC), another component of the self, and to identify neuroanatomical correlates, in prodromal to mild DLB.

Methods

Twenty patients with prodromal to mild DLB were selected to participate in this exploratory study along with 20 healthy control subjects matched in terms of age, gender, and level of education. The Twenty Statements Test (TST) was used to assess the SC. Behavioral performances were compared between LB patients and control subjects. Three-dimensional magnetic resonance images (MRI) were acquired for all participants and correlational analyses were performed using voxel-based morphometry (VBM) in whole brain and using a mask for the insula.

Results

The behavioral results on the TST showed significantly impaired performances in LB patients in comparison with control subjects (p < .0001). Correlational analyses using VBM revealed positive correlations between the TST and grey matter volume within insular cortex, right supplementary motor area, bilateral inferior temporal gyri, right inferior frontal gyrus, and left lingual gyrus, using a threshold of p = .001 uncorrected, including total intracranial volume (TIV), age, and MMSE as nuisance covariates. Additionally, correlational analysis using a mask for the insula revealed positive correlation with grey matter volume within bilateral insular cortex, using a threshold of p = .005.

Conclusions

The behavioral results confirm the existence of SC impairments in LB patients from the prodromal stage of the disease, compared to matched healthy controls. As we expected, VBM analyses revealed involvement of the insula, among that of other brain regions, already known to be involved in other self-components. While this study is exploratory, our findings provide important insights regarding the involvement of the insula within the self, confirming the insula as a core region of the self-networks, including for high-order self-representations such as the SC.

Introduction

Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) is the second most common form of cognitive neurodegenerative disease after Alzheimer’s disease (AD), with prevalence rates of up to 5% in the elderly population and up to around 20% of all cases of dementia [1, 2]. While visual hallucinations are frequently present in patients with Lewy bodies (LB), other behavioral disturbances such as anosognosia also frequently occur (Calil et al., 2021) and could be related to an alteration of the self-consciousness. DLB is characterized by insular atrophy, occurring from the early stage of the disease [3,4,5,6], the insula being a core region for the self [7].

The insula is indeed involved in a wide variety of processes related to the self, from the most internal bodily states, such as interoception [8, 9] to high-order processes such as knowledge about oneself [10], that belong to the self-concept (SC). The SC encompasses different types of self-knowledge, such as attributes, traits, beliefs, values, social status, roles, physical characteristics or even personal preferences [11, 12]. It is made up of a body of autobiographical knowledge and refers to the way we internally represent who we are, determining what sort of person we are. The SC is closely linked to autobiographical memory, and particularly to its semanticized form, which contributes to the formation and maintenance of mental representation of the self in the present moment and across time [13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20]. Thus, behavioral and neuroimaging studies are increasingly addressing tasks related to the SC, through methods exploring personal identity. In a study investigating reflective self-referential processes, healthy subjects were asked to think intensely on how they would describe their own personality traits and physical appearance. Self-referential conditions (personality traits and physical appearance) induced activation in various cortical midline structures (CMS), including the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), the anterior cingulate cortex, the supplementary motor area (SMA), and the precuneus, when compared to non-self-referential conditions (i.e., thoughts about a famous person, the Danish Queen) [21]. Another study explored brain activity whilst subjects were reflecting upon their own personal qualities as compared to those of an acquaintance. For both conditions, researchers found activations in the ACC, MPFC, SMA, and superior temporal gyrus, with significantly greater activations in the self condition. Interestingly, the insula showed unique activity associated with self-reflection, highlighting a key role for the insula in self-referential processes [10].

Hence, the aim of the present study was to better understand the impact of neurodegeneration on the SC in patients with prodromal to mild DLB, and to explore the underlying structural mechanisms, with a particular focus on the link between SC and the insula. Regarding this relationship, our team has previously highlighted the existence of modifications of the SC in LB patients from the prodromal stage, in an experiment on personal tastes. By means of a questionnaire, we demonstrated that LB patients presented significant changes in tastes in both food and non-food domains, independently of the existence of anosmia or agueusia. Moreover, we found that these changes were associated with grey matter atrophy in the insula [22], suggesting a role for the insula in supporting personal tastes as self-concepts. Based on the fact that the insula is a key region for the self and given that this region is atrophied at an early stage in LB patients, we posit that impairments of the SC would be observed in LB patients, and would be correlated to insular atrophy, among other regions involved in self-concept.

Methods

Study population

Twenty prodromal to mild DLB patients and 20 healthy control subjects matched for age, gender and level of education (with a minimum of 9 years) were enrolled in the present study between January 2021 and February 2023. A description of the study population is presented in Table 1. Patients were recruited from the tertiary memory clinic of Strasbourg University Hospital, France, including the Geriatrics and Neurology Departments. Control subjects were recruited among friends and relatives of the patients, via the listing of controls of the local clinical investigation center and from the control group of the AlphaLewyMA cohort (http://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01876459).

The LB group comprised 14 men and 6 women aged from 60 to 80 years (mean = 71.95, standard deviation [SD] = 7.99). Diagnosis of prodromal and mild DLB patients was based on core clinical features [23, 24]. Some of the patients had also benefited from biomarkers during their clinical follow-up. Indeed a dopamine transporter SPECT (DAT) scan was realized when parkinsonism was doubtful, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) analysis was realized when an amnestic syndrome of hippocampal type suggested a possible Alzheimer’s disease, to ensure that there was no co-pathology. Patients with prodromal DLB were defined as having mild cognitive impairment if they had a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [25] score ≥ 26, had preservation of independence as assessed by the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living [26], and fulfilled both the DSM-5 criteria of mild neurocognitive disorder [27] and McKeith’s criteria for the diagnosis of prodromal DLB [23]. Patients were defined as having mild DLB if they had an MMSE score between 20 and 25 and were diagnosed as having probable DLB according to the current DLB criteria [24]. Thus, 13 of the 20 patients were in the prodromal stage of DLB and the remaining seven were in the mild stage of the disease. Dopamine transporter SPECT (DAT) scan was available to support the diagnosis in approximately a quarter of the patients. CSF analysis was available to confirm that there was not associated Alzheimer’s disease in approximately half of the patients. All participants benefited from a classic medical examination, which notably included evaluation of the features of parkinsonism using the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (part 3): akinesia, rigidity, and tremor at rest (rated from 0 for no symptoms to 4 for serious symptoms). The LB group underwent further clinical examination of DLB core criteria, among which fluctuations were assessed with the Mayo Clinic Fluctuations scale [28] and the Newcastle upon Tyne Clinician Assessment of Fluctuation scale [29]. The Parkinson’s disease-associated psychotic symptoms questionnaire [30] was used to evaluate the presence of hallucinations. Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder (RBD) was evaluated using a sleep questionnaire on RBD [31], simplified into four questions each for the patient and the caregiver: one concerning movements during sleep, the second concerning vivid dreams and nightmares. Among the 20 LB patients, 85% presented fluctuations, 60% presented hallucinations or illusions, and 60% presented RBD. Concerning parkinsonism features, akinesia was observed in 80% of LB patients, rigidity in 70% of LB patients, and tremor at rest in only one patient (5%) (see Table 1).

Participants with history of alcohol/substance abuse, significant visual or auditory disabilities, relevant neurological of psychiatric comorbidities, or the presence of other severe or unstable medical illnesses were not enrolled in the study. Participants with an abnormal neurological examination – except for parkinsonism in the case of patients –, depression symptoms (mini-GDS [32]), , or a significant cerebral vascular burden (Modified Hachinski Ischemic Score scale > 7 [33]), were not enrolled. Participants with CSF biomarkers in favor of an Alzheimer’s disease (i.e. abnormal Aβ42/Aβ40 ratio, t-Tau, phospho-Tau181) were not enrolled. Finally, participants with claustrophobia or contraindications to MRI were not enrolled. All participants provided written informed consent for the study, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the study was approved by the local Ethics committee (Sud Méditerranée III).

Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics of the LB group and the control group

Behavioral study

All participants underwent an evaluation of the SC. We also assessed general cognitive functioning and verbal fluency in LB patients.

General cognitive functioning

General cognitive functioning was assessed with the MMSE, and the maximum score was 30 points.

Verbal fluency

Verbal fluency was assessed with a task in which patients had two min to generate as many words as they could beginning with the letter P. Proper nouns and variations on words (e.g., “photograph” and “photography”) were not allowed. The score was the number of correctly generated words.

Twenty statements test

The assessment of the SC was performed with the Twenty Statements Test (TST; [34]). The TST requires the subjects to make up to 20 statements in response to the question “Who am I?”. They were asked to write down each of the statements by completing the sentence “I am…”. To avoid any potential limitations due to impairment in generative strategies, cues indicating different possible categories of responses among five domains of identity (i.e., personal, family, social, moral, and physical) were given to the two groups of participants, as well as examples for each possible category of response. Instructions and cues were written down on the response sheet and an oral administration of the test was used for DLB patients in order to facilitate responses. For the purposes of this study, we only considered the number of responses provided by the participant, regardless of the category. Repeated responses and responses that did not answer the question of interest were not considered.

Statistical analyses for the behavioural study

Student’s t test was used to compare intergroup differences between LB patients and control subjects for both demographic and behavioral quantitative data. For the behavioral data, a one-tailed t test was used, since we hypothesized that LB patients would have lower scores than control subjects due to impairment of the SC. We also analyzed correlations using Pearson’s test, between the TST and MMSE scores, and between the TST and verbal fluency scores.

A chi-squared test was used to compare the sex ratio between groups. A threshold of p < .05 was used to determine statistical significance.

Neuroimaging study

Each participant underwent a high-resolution anatomical MRI scan within a maximum of 12 weeks after taking the TST. T1-weighted three-dimensional anatomical images were obtained using a 3T MRI scanner (Verio 32-channel Tim Siemens scanner; Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using a volumetric magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition with gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence (FOV = 256 × 256 mm, image matrix = 256 × 256, slice thickness = 1 mm, repetition time = 1900 ms, echo time = 2.52 ms, flip angle = 9°).

Voxel-based morphometry analyses

We used voxel-based morphometry (VBM) to investigate differences in grey matter volume between the healthy controls and the LB patients, and to examine the neuroanatomical correlates of the SC in both the healthy controls and LB patients. VBM analyses included image preprocessing and statistical analyses. These steps were carried out using the SPM12 software package (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/) running on Matlab R2017b (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). Anatomical MRI images were spatially preprocessed using standard procedures [35]. All T1-weighted structural images were first segmented, bias-corrected, and spatially normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute space using an extension of the unified segmentation procedure that includes six classes of tissue [36]. The DARTEL registration toolbox was then used to build a study-specific template and to bring into alignment all of the segmentation images. The VBM analysis was done on modulated grey matter (GM) images; that is, the GM value in each voxel was multiplied by the Jacobian determinant derived from the spatial normalization. This procedure preserves the total amount of GM from the original images. These modulated GM images were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel (full width at half maximum [FWHM]: 8 mm). Between-group voxel-based comparisons were displayed after correcting for multiple comparisons with false discovery rate (FDR; p < .05). To map the regions of atrophy related to disorders of the SC, we tested the correlation between the GM volume at a voxel level and the total score on the TST using the general linear model. Correlation analysis between behavioral data and GM volume was performed using a threshold of p = .001 uncorrected, including total intracranial volume (TIV) and age as nuisance covariates. MMSE was also considered as an additional covariate to investigate the potential impact of disease severity. A cluster spatial extent of 50 voxels was used to avoid irrelevant and isolated detections. The software Xjview (http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview8/) allowed us to characterize each cluster.

Moreover, to ensure that finding the insula was not a spurious result, when considering the number of covariates and that we did not use correction for multiple comparison, we completed the VBM analyses in whole brain, with a VBM analysis using a mask for the insula. The GM images were smoothed at 4 mm to increase specificity in order to discriminate true effect from random noise. We used a threshold of p < .005, including the same covariates as for the whole brain analyses.

Results

Behavioral study

The behavioral results indicated that the LB group had significantly decreased performances for the total score on the TST compared to the healthy controls (mean 9.7, SD 4.5 vs. 19.05, SD 3.36, t = 7.38, p < .0001). We completed correlational analyses in the LB group by examining correlations between the TST and MMSE scores, which revealed significant correlations (r = .49, p < .05). We also analyzed correlations between the TST and verbal fluency scores, which showed significant correlations (r = .55, p < .05).

Neuroimaging study

Voxel-based analysis comparing grey matter volume in LB patients versus healthy controls is presented in supplementary material. Analysis included TIV and age as nuisance covariates, and revealed patterns of cerebral atrophy typically reported in LB patients [4], with grey matter volume in insular, temporal, occipital, frontal, cingular cortices and to a lesser extent parietal cortex (p < .05, FDR corrected), in LB patients compared to healthy controls.

In the LB group, correlational analysis for the SC revealed positive correlations with GM volume within right insular cortex (see Fig. 1), right SMA (see Fig. 2), right inferior frontal gyrus, left lingual gyrus and bilateral inferior temporal gyri (see Fig. 3), using a threshold of p = .001 uncorrected, including TIV, age, and MMSE as nuisance covariates, with a minimum cluster size of k = 50 (Table 2). Correlational analysis for the SC, using a mask for the insula revealed positive correlation with GM volume within bilateral insular cortex (see Fig. 4), using a threshold of p = .005 uncorrected, including TIV, age, and MMSE as nuisance covariates. The correlational analyses did not reveal any correlation in the healthy controls.

Fig. 1
figure 1

VBM analyses for self-concept in the Lewy bodies group.Grey matter volumes within right insular cortex positively correlated with the total score on the TST questionnaire (self-concept), using a threshold of p = .001 uncorrected, including age, TIV and MMSE score as nuisance covariates, k = 50

Fig. 2
figure 2

VBM analyses for self-concept in the Lewy bodies group.Grey matter volumes within right supplementary motor area positively correlated with the total score on the TST questionnaire, using a threshold of p = .001 uncorrected, including age, TIV, and MMSE score as nuisance covariates, k = 50

Fig. 3
figure 3

VBM analyses for self-concept in the Lewy bodies group. Grey matter volumes within right insular cortex, right SMA, bilateral inferior temporal gyri, and left lingual gyrus positively correlated with the total score on the TST questionnaire, using a threshold of p = .001 uncorrected, including age, TIV, and MMSE score as nuisance covariates, k = 50

Fig. 4
figure 4

VBM analyses for self-concept in Lewy bodies group, using a mask for the insula.Grey matter volumes within bilateral insular cortex, using a threshold of p = .005 uncorrected, including age, TIV, and MMSE score as nuisance covariates

Table 2 VBM results for the total score on the Twenty Statements Test in the Lewy bodies group

Discussion

This study is the first to examine SC in LB patients by using a standardized measure. In summary, the findings indicate the existence of significant impairments of the SC in LB patients from the early stage of the disease, compared to age-matched healthy controls. Concordant with our hypothesis, imaging analyses confirmed the involvement of insular cortex atrophy in SC impairments, among other brain regions such as the SMA. These results bring new insight into the role of the insular cortex in the conception of oneself, among other brain regions more widely associated with classic self-networks.

We focused in particular on the role of the insula in the SC, with the hypothesis that insular atrophy could explain alterations of the self in LB patients. The insular cortex is commonly associated with interoceptive awareness [37] and socio-emotional processes [9]. It has also been associated with self-reflection upon personal qualities [10] and with self-esteem, two abilities that are related to the SC [38]. Regarding LB patients, our study is the first to highlight a diminished SC in relation to the insular atrophy that occurs early in the course of the disease [3,4,5,6]. SC impairments have already been linked to the atrophy of this structure in DLB, regarding modifications of personal tastes [22]. However, from the perspective of cognitive models of the self, personal preferences also imply the subjective sense of self, since liking or disliking an object is inherent to the subject in the present moment. In our study, to measure the SC in isolation from the subjective sense of self, we asked the participants to simply answer the question “who am I?”, which refers to the overall representation of oneself. In the present study, we found that both the posterior and anterior part of the insular cortex are involved in SC. The insular cortex is known to be topographically organized with a degree of complexity increasing along a posterior-anterior axis [8]. Whereas the posterior insular cortex is reported to be involved in processing a variety of bodily sensations and also in higher order somatosensory function, such as the subjective sense of body ownership and agency [39,40,41,42], the anterior insular cortex contains the representations of bodily reactions in response to affective feeling states (i.e., somatic markers) [43], sustains visual self-recognition [44, 45], and is involved in metacognitive processes [46]. Considering the involvement of the posterior insular cortex, our results suggest that this region supports conceptual aspects of the self, in addition to the most elementary aspects of subjective sense of self. One could hypothesize that the alteration of the subjective sense of self could lead to impairments of the other high order self-components such as SC, through a global collapse of the self. We posit that such damage could disrupt the feeling of what belongs to the self or to the non-self, thus making difficult the access to specific knowledge about the self. Nevertheless, since we did not focus on the subjective sense of self in the present work, our study does not confirm this hypothesis. However, it is concordant with the fact that hypoperfusion in the insula has been associated with Capgras syndrome in DLB [47], which consists of the loss of familiarity toward relatives, who are logically integrated by extension in one’s self. Concerning the anterior insular cortex, based on its role in self representations [10, 22, 38] and its involvement in metacognition [48], it appears as a main region in the conceptualization of the self. Thus, dysfunction of the anterior insular cortex could disturb the conception we have built of who we are and therefore reduce the ability to answer the question “who am I ?”.

Our second main finding concerns the relationship between CMS and the SC. In line with previous studies [49,50,51], we found that self-referential processes are associated with medial cortical regions, notably the right SMA. The SMA is not classically associated with the CMS as described by Georg Northoff. This area is responsible for planning of complex movements [52] and would sustain the sense of agency [53]. The SMA is well known to entail mirror neurons [54], described in the context of social cognition, rather than in self-cognition. Indeed, mirror neurons are visuomotor neurons discharging when we are executing a motor act or when we observe someone executing a similar motor act [55, 56]. Based on similar mechanisms, the mirror neuron system fires for feelings, such as empathy [57], and is considered to underlie theory of mind [58], which is the ability to infer and reason about another person’s mental states [59]. Theory of mind would be based on areas involved in the self brain network, such as the MPFC and the precuneus, and could be supported by representations of the self [60, 61]. Yet, the self-component associated with the SMA would rather pertain to the domain of the subjective sense of self, notably the sense of agency (e.g., when initiating a movement), whereas our study focuses on the conceptual representations of the self. From the perspective of embodied cognition, we can hypothesize that such a region able to simulate our motricity could also support high-order representations of the physical self, actually pertaining to the SC. These findings illustrate the concept of embodied cognition, as described by Binder and Desai for general semantic memory [62]. Indeed, the authors demonstrated that modality-specific systems are recruited when integrating supramodal representations, depending on the categories of representation (e.g. action words referring to face, arm, or leg activate frontocentral motor areas according to a somatotopic organization [63]). Concordant with this hypothesis, it has been demonstrated that mirror-neuron areas are engaged in tasks of self-face recognition [64] and would enable physical other-to-self mapping [65]. Thus, these specific neurons could play a major role in identification and representation of the physical self, which is part of the SC.

Aside from the involvement of insula and CMS, we also found correlations between impairments of the SC and diminished grey matter volume in bilateral inferior temporal gyri – including right fusiform gyrus –, left lingual gyrus and right inferior frontal gyrus. Interestingly, it has been proposed that the inferior temporal gyri, notably the anterior part, might play a key role in semantic memory [66]. In our study, we found correlations in the posterior part, in LB patients, which could be associated with the personal aspects of semantic knowledge. More specifically, the right fusiform gyrus is known to be specialized in face perception [67, 68]. Additionally, Kircher et al. found involvement of the left fusiform gyrus in personality-traits describing the self and perception of own face [45], suggesting a strong involvement of this region in self-processing. Regarding the lingual gyrus, it is a region essential for visual perception [69] and visual memory [70]. Likewise, the lingual gyrus plays a central role in divergent thinking [71], which refers to the processes allowing one to generate creative ideas by exploring different possibilities. We could speculate that divergent thinking is involved in the creation of oneself prototype. Interestingly, we found diminished grey matter volume in the right inferior frontal gyrus, which has been related to verbal fluency [72]. Yet, the TST score and verbal fluency were positively correlated in our study and could reflect research strategies in memory. Another ability that is necessary to describe one’s own SC is introspection, which has been associated with inferior frontal gyrus by means of multiband functional MRI during structured real-time conversations, in which the participant verbalized introspected thought and feeling [73]. Impairments in self-introspection could explain a poorer SC, and even be linked to impairments in introspection of consciousness, related to anosognosia. Indeed, introspection is closely linked to metacognition which is part of the subjective sense of self [18]. Subjective sense of self and SC are two interrelated concepts and distinguishing between them represents a real experimental challenge.

Overall, our study has some limitations. First, the sample sizes were relatively small (N = 20 in each group) with a high number of covariates and the results for the VBM analyses were uncorrected. These preliminary results should therefore be interpreted carefully and be confirmed with additional studies, involving a larger cohort of DLB patients. Moreover, comparing the SC across different clinical populations (e.g. fronto-temporal lobar degeneration, AD) at the same stage would highlight the impact of different neurodegenerative processes on the SC. Second, it would have been useful to assess and integrate other components of the self, such as subjective sense of self and autobiographical semantic memory, as additional covariates, in order to better isolate the SC. Therefore, our future work aims to investigate the relationship between the subjective sense of self, the SC, and autobiographical memory in both DLB and AD, with a special focus on the insula’s role within the different components.

Conclusions

Our work on the SC in DLB has both clinical and theoretical interest. First, it is the first study to demonstrate the existence of SC impairments in LB patients. Concordant with our hypothesis, these difficulties are related to insular atrophy, which occurs early in the course of the disease. Moreover, our study confirms the involvement of the SMA in the self, and notably suggest a role for mirror neurons. Finally, we found that bilateral inferior temporal gyri, right inferior frontal gyrus and left lingual gyrus also play a role in the SC. This finding brings new insights regarding the implication of the insula in the different aspects of the self. Our results show that the SC relies on the insula and other brain regions involved in the other components of the self, such as the subjective sense of self. These findings suggest that conceptual representations of the self could arise from the primary phenomenological levels.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

DLB:

Dementia with Lewy bodies

LB:

Lewy bodies

SC:

Self-concept

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

AD:

Alzheimer’s disease

CMS:

Cortical midline structures

SMA:

Supplementary motor area

SD:

Standard deviation

DAT:

Dopamine transporter SPECT

CSF:

Cerebrospinal fluid

MMSE:

Mini-Mental State Examination

RBD:

Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder

TST:

Twenty Statements Test

VBM:

Voxel-based morphometry

GM:

Grey matter

FDR:

False discovery rate

TIV:

Total intracranial volume

References

  1. McKeith IG, Dickson DW, Lowe J, Emre M, O’Brien JT, Feldman H, et al. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: third report of the DLB consortium. Neurology. 2005;65:1863–72. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000187889.17253.b1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Zaccai J, McCracken C, Brayne C. A systematic review of prevalence and incidence studies of dementia with Lewy bodies. Age Ageing. 2005;34:561–6. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afi190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Blanc F, Noblet V, Philippi N, Cretin B, Foucher J, Armspach J-P, et al. Right anterior insula: Core Region of Hallucinations in Cognitive Neurodegenerative diseases. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e114774. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114774.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Blanc F, Colloby SJ, Philippi N, de Pétigny X, Jung B, Demuynck C, et al. Cortical thickness in dementia with Lewy Bodies and Alzheimer’s Disease: a comparison of Prodromal and Dementia stages. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0127396. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0127396.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Blanc F, Colloby SJ, Cretin B, de Sousa PL, Demuynck C, O’Brien JT, et al. Grey matter atrophy in prodromal stage of dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2016;8:31. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-016-0198-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Roquet D, Noblet V, Anthony P, Philippi N, Demuynck C, Cretin B, et al. Insular atrophy at the prodromal stage of dementia with Lewy bodies: a VBM DARTEL study. Sci Rep. 2017;7:9437. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-08667-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Tisserand A, Philippi N, Botzung A, Blanc F, Me. Myself and my insula: an Oasis in the forefront of self-consciousness. Biology. 2023;12:599. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology12040599.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Craig AD. How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:59–70. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Damasio A. Feelings of emotion and the self. Ann N Acad Sci. 2003;1001:253–61. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1279.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Modinos G, Ormel J, Aleman A. Activation of anterior insula during self-reflection. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:4618. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004618.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Carver CS. Self-awareness. In: Leary MR, Tangney JP, editors. Handb. Self Identity. New-York, NY: Guilford Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Morin A, Everett J. Inner speech as a mediator of self-awareness, self-consciousness, and self-knowledge: an hypothesis. New Ideas Psychol. 1990;8:337–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Addis DR, McIntosh AR, Moscovitch M, Crawley AP, McAndrews MP. Characterizing spatial and temporal features of autobiographical memory retrieval networks: a partial least squares approach. NeuroImage. 2004;23:1460–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Addis DR, Tippett LJ. The contributions of autobiographical memory to the content and continuity of self: a social-cognitive neuro-science approach. In: Fabio S, editor. Self-Contin. Individ. Collect. Perspect. New-York, NY: Psychology; 2008. pp. 71–84.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Conway MA, Singer JA, Tagini A. The self and autobiographical memory: correspondence and coherence. Soc Cogn. 2004;22:491–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Kihlstrom JF, Beer JS, Klein SB. Self and identity as memory. In: Leary MR, Tangney JP, editors. Handb. Self Identity. New-York, NY: Guilford Press; 2003. pp. 68–90.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Klein SB, Loftus J. The mental representation of trait and autobiographical knowledge about the self. In: Srull TK, Wyer RS, editors. Adv. Soc. Cogn., vol. 5, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1993.

  18. Prebble SC, Addis DR, Tippett LJ. Autobiographical memory and sense of self. Psychol Bull. 2013;139:815–40. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Rathbone CJ, Moulin CJ, Conway MA. Self-centered memories: the reminiscence bump and the self. Mem Cogn. 2008;36:1403–14. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.8.1403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Wilson AE, Ross M. The identity function of autobiographical memory: time is on our side. Memory. 2003;11:137–49. https://doi.org/10.1080/741938210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Kjaer TW, Nowak M, Lou HC. Reflective self-awareness and Conscious States: PET evidence for a common midline Parietofrontal Core. NeuroImage. 2002;17:1080–6. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Philippi N, Noblet V, Hamdaoui M, Soulier D, Botzung A, Ehrhard E, et al. The insula, a grey matter of tastes: a volumetric MRI study in dementia with Lewy bodies. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2020;12:79. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-020-00645-y.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. McKeith IG, Ferman TJ, Thomas AJ, Blanc F, Boeve BF, Fujishiro H, et al. Research criteria for the diagnosis of prodromal dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology. 2020;94:743–55. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000009323.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. McKeith IG, Boeve BF, Dickson DW, Halliday G, Taylor J-P, Weintraub D, et al. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: fourth consensus report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology. 2017;89:88–100. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000004058.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of Older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of Daily Living. Gerontologist. 1969;9:179–86. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. American Psychiatric Association, American Psychiatric Association, editors. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  28. Ferman TJ, Smith GE, Boeve BF, Ivnik RJ, Petersen RC, Knopman D, et al. DLB fluctuations: specific features that reliably differentiate DLB from AD and normal aging. Neurology. 2004;62:181–7. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.62.2.181.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Walker MP, Ayre GA, Cummings JL, Wesnes K, McKeith IG, O’Brien JT, et al. The Clinician Assessment of fluctuation and the one day fluctuation Assessment Scale: two methods to assess fluctuating confusion in dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;177:252–6. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.177.3.252.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Fénelon G, Soulas T, Zenasni F, de Langavant LC. The changing face of Parkinson’s disease-associated psychosis: a cross-sectional study based on the new NINDS-NIMH criteria. Mov Disord. 2010;25:763–6. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.22839.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Gjerstad MD, Boeve B, Wentzel-Larsen T, Aarsland D, Larsen JP. Occurrence and clinical correlates of REM sleep behaviour disorder in patients with Parkinson’s disease over time. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008;79:387–91. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2007.116830.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Clément JP, Nassif RF, Léger JM, Marchan F. [Development and contribution to the validation of a brief French version of the Yesavage Geriatric Depression Scale]. L’Encephale. 1997;23:91–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Hachinski VC, Iliff LD, Zilhka E, Du Boulay GH, McAllister VL, Marshall J, et al. Cerebral blood flow in dementia. Arch Neurol. 1975;32:632–7. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1975.00490510088009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Kuhn MH, McPartland TS. An empirical investigation of self-attitudes. Am Sociol Rev. 1954;19:68. https://doi.org/10.2307/2088175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RNA, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RSJ. A voxel-based morphometric study of Ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage. 2001;14:21–36. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0786.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. NeuroImage. 2005;26:839–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Craig AD. Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:500–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00090-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Eisenberger NI, Inagaki TK, Muscatell KA, Byrne Haltom KE, Leary MR. The neural sociometer: brain mechanisms underlying state self-esteem. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:3448–55. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Farrer C, Franck N, Georgieff N, Frith CD, Decety J, Jeannerod M. Modulating the experience of agency: a positron emission tomography study. NeuroImage. 2003;18:324–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00041-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Schulz SM. Neural correlates of heart-focused interoception: a functional magnetic resonance imaging meta-analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;371. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0018.

  41. Stephan E, Pardo JV, Faris PL, Hartman BK, Kim SW, Ivanov EH, et al. Functional neuroimaging of gastric distention. J Gastrointest Surg. 2003;7:740–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1091-255x(03)00071-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Tsakiris M, Hesse MD, Boy C, Haggard P, Fink GR. Neural signatures of body ownership: a sensory network for bodily self-consciousness. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2235–44. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhl131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Damasio A. Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. Putnam, NY: G.P. Putnam; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Devue C, Collette F, Balteau E, Degueldre C, Luxen A, Maquet P, et al. Here I am: the cortical correlates of visual self-recognition. Brain Res. 2007;1143:169–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.055.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Kircher TT, Senior C, Phillips ML, Benson PJ, Bullmore ET, Brammer M, et al. Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self processing: effects of faces and words. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2000;10:133–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00036-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Fleming SM, Dolan RJ. The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2012;367:1338–49. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Nagahama Y, Okina T, Suzuki N, Matsuda M. Neural correlates of psychotic symptoms in dementia with Lewy bodies. Brain. 2010;133:557–67. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Sinanaj I, Cojan Y, Vuilleumier P. Inter-individual variability in metacognitive ability for visuomotor performance and underlying brain structures. Conscious Cogn. 2015;36:327–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.07.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. D’Argembeau A, Ruby P, Collette F, Degueldre C, Balteau E, Luxen A, et al. Distinct regions of the medial prefrontal cortex are associated with self-referential processing and perspective taking. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:935–44. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.935.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. D’Argembeau A, Stawarczyk D, Majerus S, Collette F, Van der Linden M, Salmon E. Modulation of medial prefrontal and inferior parietal cortices when thinking about past, present, and future selves. Soc Neurosci. 2010;5:187–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910903233562.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Northoff G, Bermpohl F. Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8:102–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Deiber M-P, Passingham RE, Colebatch JG, Friston KJ, Nixon PD, Frackowiak RSJ. Cortical areas and the selection of movement: a study with positron emission tomography. Exp Brain Res. 1991;84. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00231461.

  53. Moore JW, Ruge D, Wenke D, Rothwell J, Haggard P. Disrupting the experience of control in the human brain: pre-supplementary motor area contributes to the sense of agency. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2010;277:2503–9. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Mukamel R, Ekstrom AD, Kaplan J, Iacoboni M, Fried I. Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and Observation of actions. Curr Biol. 2010;20:750–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Jacob P, Jeannerod M. The motor theory of social cognition: a critique. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:21–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Rizzolatti G, Craighero L, THE MIRROR-NEURON, SYSTEM. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:169–92. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Iacoboni M. Neural mechanisms of imitation. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005;15:632–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Siegal M, Varley R. Neural systems involved in theory of mind. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:463–71. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn844.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Premack D, Woodruff G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci. 1978;1:515–26. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Happé F. Theory of mind and the self. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1001:134–44. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1279.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Saxe R. Theory of Mind (Neural Basis). Encycl. Conscious., Elsevier; 2009, pp. 401–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012373873-8.00078-5.

  62. Binder JR, Desai RH. The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:527–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Hauk O, Johnsrude I, Pulvermüller F. Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex. Neuron. 2004;41:301–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Uddin LQ, Kaplan JT, Molnar-Szakacs I, Zaidel E, Iacoboni M. Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal mirror network in the right hemisphere: an event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2005;25:926–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Uddin LQ, Iacoboni M, Lange C, Keenan JP. The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:153–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.01.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Chan D, Fox NC, Scahill RI, Crum WR, Whitwell JL, Leschziner G, et al. Patterns of temporal lobe atrophy in semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol. 2001;49:433–42. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Bruce V, Young A. Understanding face recognition. Br J Psychol. 1986;77:305–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb02199.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception. J Neurosci. 1997;17:4302–11. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Yang Y, Deng H, Xing G, Xia X, Li H. Brain functional network connectivity based on a visual task: visual information processing-related brain regions are significantly activated in the task state. Neural Regen Res. 2015;10:298. https://doi.org/10.4103/1673-5374.152386.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Bogousslavsky J, Miklossy J, Deruaz JP, Assal G, Regli F. Lingual and fusiform gyri in visual processing: a clinico-pathologic study of superior altitudinal hemianopia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1987;50:607–14. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.50.5.607.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Zhang L, Qiao L, Chen Q, Yang W, Xu M, Yao X, et al. Gray Matter volume of the Lingual Gyrus mediates the relationship between inhibition function and divergent thinking. Front Psychol. 2016;7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01532.

  72. Katzorke A, Zeller JBM, Müller LD, Lauer M, Polak T, Reif A, et al. Reduced activity in the right Inferior Frontal Gyrus in Elderly APOE-E4 carriers during a Verbal Fluency Task. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017;11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00046.

  73. Yoshioka A, Tanabe HC, Nakagawa E, Sumiya M, Koike T, Sadato N. The role of the Left Inferior Frontal Gyrus in Introspection during Verbal Communication. Brain Sci. 2023;13:111. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13010111.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful for financial support provided by the Association France Alzheimer et Maladies Apparentées, the Association Vaincre Alzheimer and the Strasbourg University Foundation. We are also indebted to the Clinical Investigation Center of Strasbourg for its support in the recruitment of participants. We warmly thank all the patients, the controls, and their relatives for participating in this study. We also thank Nick Barton for language editing.

Funding

Association France Alzheimer et Maladie Apparentées, Association Vaincre Alzheimer, Strasbourg University Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Study conception and design (N.P., F.B., A.T.) and study coordination (N.P., F.B., A.T., A.B.); acquisition of clinical data (A.T., N.P., F.B., C.M., H.D., C.D., A.R.); acquisition of MRI data (A.T., P. L. S.); extraction of clinical data (L.S.); manuscript preparation (A.T., N.P.); critical revision of the manuscript (N.P., F.B.); imaging and statistical analysis (A.T., M.M.). All authors reviewed and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alice Tisserand.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The local ethics committee approved the study protocol (“Comité de Protection des Personnes Sud Méditerranée III”; approval number 2020-A02475-34). All participants provided written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tisserand, A., Blanc, F., Mondino, M. et al. Who am I with my Lewy bodies? The insula as a core region of the self-concept networks. Alz Res Therapy 16, 85 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-024-01447-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-024-01447-2

Keywords