Skip to main content

Purpose in life promotes resilience to age-related brain burden in middle-aged adults

Abstract

Background

Disease-modifying agents to counteract cognitive impairment in older age remain elusive. Hence, identifying modifiable factors promoting resilience, as the capacity of the brain to maintain cognition and function with aging and disease, is paramount. In Alzheimer’s disease (AD), education and occupation are typical cognitive reserve proxies. However, the importance of psychological factors is being increasingly recognized, as their operating biological mechanisms are elucidated. Purpose in life (PiL), one of the pillars of psychological well-being, has previously been found to reduce the deleterious effects of AD-related pathological changes on cognition. However, whether PiL operates as a resilience factor in middle-aged individuals and what are the underlying neural mechanisms remain unknown.

Methods

Data was obtained from 624 middle-aged adults (mean age 53.71 ± 6.9; 303 women) from the Barcelona Brain Health Initiative cohort. Individuals with lower (LP; N = 146) and higher (HP; N = 100) PiL rates, according to the division of this variable into quintiles, were compared in terms of cognitive status, a measure reflecting brain burden (white matter lesions; WMLs), and resting-state functional connectivity, examining system segregation (SyS) parameters using 14 common brain circuits.

Results

Neuropsychological status and WMLs burden did not differ between the PiL groups. However, in the LP group, greater WMLs entailed a negative impact on executive functions. Subjects in the HP group showed lower SyS of the dorsal default-mode network (dDMN), indicating lesser segregation of this network from other brain circuits. Specifically, HP individuals had greater inter-network connectivity between specific dDMN nodes, including the frontal cortex, the hippocampal formation, the midcingulate region, and the rest of the brain. Greater functional connectivity in some of these nodes positively correlated with cognitive performance.

Conclusion

Expanding previous findings on AD pathology and advanced age, the present results suggest that higher rates of PiL may promote resilience against brain changes already observable in middle age. Furthermore, having a purposeful life implies larger functional integration of the dDMN, which may potentially reflect greater brain reserve associated to better cognitive function.

Introduction

Increased life expectancy represents one of the biggest transformations of age structure among contemporary societies [1]. This denotes an important accomplishment but also poses an enormous societal and health challenge, as advanced age is a principal risk factor for many highly prevalent and disabling disorders, including Alzheimer’s disease (AD). In this context, and despite intense research efforts and large investments from public and private sources, disease-modifying agents to counteract age-related cognitive impairment remain elusive. Given the lack of effective therapeutic interventions, identifying modifiable factors that may promote brain health and resilience along the lifespan is paramount [2,3,4].

According to the recent collaborative framework definitions (https://reserveandresilience.com/), resilience refers to a general term reflecting the capacity of the brain to maintain cognition and function with advancing age and disease. Within this framework, the investigation of resilience mechanisms can be approximated through the operational definitions of cognitive reserve (CR), which for experimental and observational studies recommend the inclusion of at least three main components: (1) a measure of brain change, (2) a measure of cognition, and (3) a variable that influences the relationship between 1 and 2. The latter variable or variables, which are often referred as “proxies of CR,” have classically been derived from single or composite estimations of educational attainment, occupation, or engagement in leisure activities, with overall converging evidences that higher estimations of these measures allow to counteract the impact of pathology or age-related changes on cognitive outcomes [5, 6]. More recently, other modifiable factors, such as cognitive and physical activities [7, 8], combined mental and bodily practices including meditation and yoga [9,10,11], sleep patterns (i.e., [12, 13]), and dietary approaches [14, 15], are also being considered as modifiable lifestyles that may promote resilience. This also includes psychological factors, which are being increasingly recognized for their contribution to brain health during the lifespan [16, 17], and as their biological substrates are clarified [18, 19]. In this context, purpose in life (PiL), included in Ryff’s prevalent model of psychological well-being [20,21,22], is one of the most promising psychological constructs. PiL refers to the sense that life has meaning and direction and that one’s goals and potential are being achieved or are attainable [23].

Modern conceptions of PiL stem from the philosophical writings of existential philosophy, which has its formal beginning on the dissertations published in the 1840s by Sören Kierkegaard. It is relevant to consider that, while philosophers have debated whether meaning exists and what its contingencies might be, psychologists and neuroscientists have primarily focused on the importance of experiencing meaning and purpose in one’s life, exploring how this links with human health [24]. Hence, it has been previously highlighted that having a strong sense of meaning and future-oriented goals may result in a greater capacity to tolerate challenging situations for mental and physical health [25, 26]. Moreover, recent literature has revealed that having a purposeful life is associated with the maintenance of health-promoting behaviors in advanced age [27], better use of preventive health services [28], and reduced odds of mortality [23, 29]. Furthermore, PiL relates to better cognitive and affective status [16, 30] and may delay cognitive decline and the onset of cognitive impairment [31,32,33], thus potentially having a role as a resilience factor against AD-related pathological changes [34]. Interestingly, PiL is a transcultural conception. In the Japanese culture, Ikigai refers to a broader concept than PiL, which has been associated with longevity and lower risk of developing functional disability and dementia [35, 36]. However, the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the beneficial effects of having a purposeful life remain poorly understood.

In this study, we aimed to investigate the relationships between PiL, common brain changes occurring in adulthood, brain functional connectivity, and cognition in a sample of healthy middle-aged individuals. More specifically, and according to the operational definition of CR described above, our first objective was to study whether PiL can influence the relationship between white matter lesions (WMLs) and cognitive status in middle-aged adults, in a similar manner as observed in pathological aging [34]. Our secondary aim was to delineate the functional brain mechanisms that could explain the expected protective effects of PiL in the face of WMLs, one of the most prevalent structural brain changes in maturity, present in almost 95% of adults over 45 years of age [37,38,39,40]. For this purpose, a measure of system segregation (SyS) was used to interrogate the dynamic neural mechanisms associated with PiL. This metric was chosen because it condenses in a single estimate of the functional connectivity of within and between brain networks’ nodes [41]. Furthermore, this measure has also been used to study brain networks across the healthy adult lifespan [42] as well as resilience in the face of AD pathology [43]. Importantly, the study of the factors and brain mechanisms providing resilience to WMLs is also of interest in the context of dementia, since these are associated with vascular and amyloid pathology in aging [44, 45], and can predict faster cognitive decline and the appearance of earlier clinical manifestations [46, 47]. Since the core components of PiL, such as the identification of personal values, are modifiable through psychological interventions (e.g., [48]), identifying the functional brain signatures through which its effects operate can provide a relevant mechanistic understanding of early interventions to enhance brain health and may even prevent dementia.

Methods

Participants

Data was obtained from 624 middle-aged adults (mean age 53.71 ± 6.9 years; age range: 42–67; 303 women; mean years of education [YoE] 17.1 ± 3.8 years, YoE range: 8–34) from the Barcelona Brain Health Initiative (BBHI; https://bbhi.cat/en/). This is an ongoing longitudinal cohort study investigating the determinants of brain and mental health in middle-aged individuals [3, 49]. For the present work, participants were included if they met the following criteria: (i) completed a PiL questionnaire (see below) and a neuropsychological assessment and (ii) T1- and T2-weighted and resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) scans were available. Participants were excluded if they (i) had any neurological or psychiatric diagnosis, (ii) had below normative data on any of the administered neuropsychological tests, (iii) imaging quality check was not satisfactory, and (iv) had statistically extreme values on WMLs, based on the Shapiro-Wilk test (p > 0.05 [50, 51]) and a visual inspection of the box plots.

Assessment of purpose in life

PiL was measured through an online self-administered questionnaire using Ryff’s Psychological Well-Being Scale [20]. Within the BBHI framework, this scale was also used to assess personal growth. Additionally, access to further well-being measures was available (please, see [3]). However, in the present study, we only focused on the PiL dimension, as previous investigations have particularly revealed its potential to confer resilience to brain burden (i.e., [34]). In the PiL questionnaire, participants reported from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) the following questions: “I live life one day at a time and don’t really think about the future”; “I have a sense of direction and purpose in life”; “I don’t have a good sense of what it is I’m trying to accomplish in life”; “My daily activities often seem trivial and unimportant to me”; “I enjoy making plans for the future and working to make them a reality”; “Some people wander aimlessly through life, but I am not one of them”; and “I sometimes feel as if I’ve done all there is to do in life.” Direct and inverse items were controlled to obtain a total sum score per participant. All included participants answered the complete number of questionnaire items.

Neuropsychological assessment

Neuropsychological testing was administered by expert neuropsychologists in a single session of approximately 90 min [3, 17]. Tests battery followed a fixed order and included direct and inverse Digit Span [52], Trail Making Test parts A and B [52], Reasoning Matrix [53], Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test [54], Block Design Test [53], Letter-Number Sequencing [52], Digit-Symbol Substitution Test and Cancellation subtests from WAIS-IV [53], and Corsi block-tapping test [52].

MRI acquisition

MRI data were acquired in a 3-T Siemens scanner (MAGNETOM Prisma) with a 32-channel head coil at Unitat d’Imatge per Ressonància Magnètica IDIBAPS (Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer) at Hospital Clínic de Barcelona, Barcelona. For all participants, a high-resolution T1-weighted structural image was obtained with a magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) three-dimensional protocol (repetition time [TR] = 2400 ms, echo time [TE] = 2.22 ms, inversion time = 1000 ms, field of view [FOV] = 256 mm, flip angle = 8° and 0.8-mm isotropic voxel). Additionally, a high-resolution 3D SPC T2-weighted structural brain MRI was undertaken (TR = 3200 ms, TE = 563 ms, flip angle = 120°, 0.8-mm isotropic voxel, FOV = 256 mm). They also underwent rs-fMRI multiband (anterior-posterior phase-encoding; acceleration factor = 8) interleaved acquisitions (T2-weighted EPI scans, TR = 800 ms, TE = 37 ms, 750 volumes, 72 slices, slice thickness = 2 mm, FOV = 208 mm). All MRI images were examined by a senior neuroradiologist for any clinically significant pathology and visually inspected by trained MRI technicians for subjective quality control of metallic or motion artifacts. To control for movement between rs-fMRI scans, the framewise displacement (FWD) mean was computed.

Image analyses

The FMRIB Software Library (FSL, version 5.0.11; https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/), Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM, version 12; https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/), and FreeSurfer (version 6.0; https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) were used for preprocessing and analyzing MRI data. Preprocessing pipeline and head movement considerations are described in SM.

White matter burden calculation

Structural T1- and T2-weighted images were automatically processed with FreeSurfer generating white matter hypointensities (WMHs) and estimated total intracranial volume (ETICV) values. Then WMHs were divided by ETICV to obtain an estimate of WMLs adjusted to head volume. This automated method was used as white matter hypointensities and hyperintensities have shown equivalent correlations with age and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) β-amyloid in non-demented elderly subjects [55]. Moreover, this methodology is strongly associated with other processes used to capture WMLs burden and the Fazekas score [56] and might avoid the potential inclusion of transient lesions [57, 58].

Resting-state functional connectivity (rs-FC) analyses

A node-based approach was adopted to quantify individual functional connectivity of resting-state networks (RSNs) as defined in the Shirer atlas of 90 nodes and 14 networks [59]. Blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal was extracted and averaged across all voxels falling within each region of interest (ROI). Then, ROI-to-ROI rs-FCs were computed as Pearson correlations and subsequently Fisher-Z transformed. Then, rs-FCs values were included in the calculation of SyS [41], a versatile graph theory-based measure of functional brain network integrity. For this purpose, negative values of rs-FC were set to 0, and autocorrelations were not considered. SyS values of each of the 14 networks were calculated as expressed in:

$${\textrm{SyS}}_{\textrm{net}}=\frac{W_{\textrm{net}}-{B}_{\textrm{net}}}{W_{\textrm{net}}}$$

SySnet captures the balance between within-network (Wnet) and between-networks (Bnet) rs-FC. Wnet was computed as the average rs-FC connecting all the nodes within the same network, while Bnet was computed as the average rs-FC connecting nodes of a network to nodes from the rest of the brain. Note that two subjects were not considered on the primary visual SyS due to issues during the rs-fMRI processing.

Statistical analyses

Data analyses were performed using IBM SPSS (IBM Corp. Released 2020. IBM SPSS Statistics, version 27.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp) and GraphPad Prism (version 9.0.0, GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA, USA).

First, the total sample was stratified to create two extreme groups, in a similar manner as previous PiL investigations (i.e., [23, 32,33,34]). With this aim, we used the “visual binning” function from the SPSS, which divides all the included subjects according to a specified number of cut points. The option “equal percentiles based on scanned cases” was used to create 5 sub-groups: Q1 (PiL values: ≤ 21; N = 146), Q2 (PiL values: 22–25, N = 135), Q3 (PiL values: 26–27, N = 114), Q4 (PiL values: 28–30, N = 129), and Q5 (PiL values: ≥ 31, N = 100). We later conducted analyses focusing on the extreme groups: the Q1, named lower PiL (LP) group, and the Q5, named higher PiL (HP) group. These two groups shaped our sample of interest, conformed by 246 individuals. Basic demographic data (age, gender, YoE) was directly compared between the PiL groups through a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and a chi-squared test. Cognitive data was integrated into three composite scores: an episodic memory composite (EMc), an executive functioning composite (EFc), and a working memory composite (WMc). The EMc was calculated considering the three recall measures from the Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test (immediate, delayed, and recognition). The EFc measure was computed considering the Reasoning Matrix, Block Design Test, Digit-Symbol Substitution Test and Cancellation subtests from WAIS-IV. The WMc measure was calculated with the inverse Digit Span and the Letter-Number Sequencing. Composites were obtained through factorial analyses with SPSS. Brain burden was evaluated considering the total estimation of WMLs. rs-fMRI analyses were computed using SyS data on the 14 Shirer circuits [59]. To investigate the neuropsychological differences between the PiL groups, a multivariate general linear model (GLM) was conducted considering all cognitive measures together as dependent variables. Moreover, a univariate GLM with WMLs as the dependent variable was calculated to study the brain burden group differences. Subsequently, Pearson correlations between cognitive status and WMLs in each group, as well as slope differences between the groups, were computed. This latter analysis was conducted with regression functions from GraphPad Prism. In addition, to investigate rs-fMRI differences, a multivariate GLM was undertaken considering the 14 Shirer networks altogether. Whether significant group differences emerged on a whole functional system, a subordinate zoom-in was conducted focused on its ROI-to-ROI functional couplings through multiple GLM analyses, considering within- and between-brain network connectivity. As per its exploratory nature, these analyses were not corrected for multiple comparisons. Finally, Pearson correlations were calculated to relate rs-fMRI measures with cognitive performance in each group. In all the stated statistical analyses, age and gender were used as covariates. YoE were included as a covariate when cognitive data was examined. Moreover, all rs-fMRI explorations were also controlled for FWD. All statistical analyses were two-tailed, and α was set at 0.05. For the Pearson correlation analyses, a bootstrapping with 5000 samples was also applied, and the bias-corrected and accelerated 95% confidence interval (CI) was reported. Quality checks were conducted using the stated covariates to corroborate whether the main associations were also present in the whole sample, as well as to further investigate the role of specific variables (i.e., age). Data in plots are presented with standardized Z scores, considering the main variables (cognition, WMLs, rs-fMRI) as well as the covariates included in each model (age, gender, YoE, FWD).

Results

Neuropsychological and WMLs analyses

Neuropsychological status and WMLs did not differ between the HP and LP groups (all p-values > 0.05). However, the slopes between the groups (HP vs. LP) were significantly different in the association between EFc and WMLs (F = 9.957, p = 0.002). Greater WMLs were negatively associated with EFc in the LP group (r = − 0.283, p < 0.001, 95% CI [− 0.405, − 0.156]) but not in the HP group (r = 0.120, p = 0.243, 95% CI [− 0.093, 0.324]; Fig. 1). No significant results were observed for the other cognitive measures (all p-values > 0.05). As a quality check, in the whole sample, the negative association between EFc and WMLs was also present (r = − 0.137, p < 0.001, 95% CI [− 0.211, − 0.060]). Moreover, age, which did not differ between the groups (p > 0.05; Additional file 1: Table S1), was positively associated with WMLs in both groups (HP group: r = 0.345, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.169, 0.499]; LP group: r = 0.384, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.223, 0.525]). Demographic data (age, gender, YoE) in each PiL group is further displayed in Additional file 1: Table S1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Associations between brain burden and cognition. Scatter plot showing the association between WMLs and executive performance as a function of the PiL group. Data is presented with Z scores. Abbreviations: EFc, executive functioning composite; HP, higher purpose in life; LP, lower power in life; WMLs, white matter lesions

rs-fMRI analyses

Subjects in the HP group showed lower SyS on the dorsal default-mode network than LP subjects (F = 4.907, p = 0.028), which indicated lesser segregation of this network from other brain circuits. Specifically, HP individuals had greater inter-network connectivity between dDMN nodes and the rest of the brain on the functional couplings depicted in Fig. 2A (see also Additional file 1: Table S2). It is worth noting that, out of the total functional connections identified in this subsequent analysis, three nodes were central: the right superior frontal node, which involved 15 out of 45 of the couplings (33.3%); the hippocampal formation, implicated in 14 out of 45 connections (31.1%), particularly the right hippocampus; and the midcingulate cortex, present in 8 out of 45 functional bridges (17.8%). Altogether, these functional hubs were present in 37 of the 45 functional connections, explaining 82.2% of the results. Conversely, subjects in the LP group exhibited more connectivity than HP individuals across DMN-like regions, particularly those involving connections between the dDMN and the ventral DMN (vDMN; Fig. 2B; Additional file 1: Table S2). The stated DMN-focused ROI-to-ROI analyses comprised a total of 765 comparisons. Of note, no differences were detected in the dDMN between-network connectivity with the other brain circuits nor in the connectivity within the dDMN nodes. Hence, our SyS results were driven by specific dDMN hubs with enhanced inter-connectivity with other brain regions. As a quality check, the negative association between PiL and dDMN SyS was also detected in the whole sample (r = − 0.081, p = 0.044, 95% CI [− 0.158, − 0.001]).

Fig. 2
figure 2

rs-fMRI contrasts between the HP and LP groups. Representation on a standard map of the significant connections between the dDMN nodes and the rest of the brain for A HP > LP and B HP < LP comparisons. The node size has been generated according to the relative number of edges in each contrast. Abbreviations: AN, attentional network; AS, anterior salience network; AUD, auditory network; BG, basal ganglia network; dDMN, dorsal default-mode network; HP, higher purpose in life; hVis, high visual network; LANG, language network; LECN, left executive control network; LP, lower purpose in life; PREC, precuneus network; prVIS, primary visual network; PS, posterior salience network; RECN, right executive control network; SM, sensorimotor network; SyS system segregation; vDMN, ventral default-mode network

Associations between neuropsychological and rs-fMRI data

Finally, we investigated whether rs-fMRI might differently subtend cognitive function for a given brain burden estimation as a function of the PiL group. Considering the HP > LP identified couplings, we detected two functional connections positively associated with executive performance in the HP group. Both connections involved the midcingulate region of the dDMN and two other cognitive brain systems, the posterior salience (PS), within its thalamic node (r = 0.205; p = 0.045, 95% CI [0.021, 0.390]; Fig. 3A), and the precuneus network, within its midcingulate and posterior cingulate cortices (r = 0.206, p = 0.044, 95% CI [0.019, 0.374]; Fig. 3B). No associations with cognition were observed in the LP group (all p-values > 0.05). These correlations survived when controlling for WMLs (r = 0.204, p = 0.047, 95% CI [0.015, 0.378]; r = 0.214, p = 0.037, 95% CI [0.042, 0.373], respectively). Furthermore, greater values on the variables used in this analysis (EFc and rs-fMRI) were not directly related to WMLs (all p-values > 0.05).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Associations between cognition and rs-FC in the HP group. Scatter plots showing the relationships in the HP group between executive functions and rs-FC between the midcingulate cortex node of the dDMN and A the left thalamus node of the posterior salience network and B the midcingulate and posterior cingulate node of the precuneus network. Data is presented with Z scores. Abbreviations: dDMN, dorsal default-mode network; EFc, executive functioning composite; midCC, midcingulate cortex; pCC, posterior cingulate cortex; PREC, precuneus network; PS, posterior salience network; thal, thalamus

Discussion

In the present study, we first observed that individuals in the highest PiL quintile showed less impact of WMLs on executive functions as compared to those in the lower PiL quintile, indicating greater resilience of HP individuals to brain damage. Subsequently, data revealed that HP participants exhibited lower SyS of the dDMN as compared to LP subjects. At the topographic level, this fact was associated with greater inter-network connectivity between specific dDMN nodes and the rest of the brain, mainly including the frontal cortex, the hippocampal formation, and the midcingulate region. Remarkably, some of these functional couplings supported cognition in the HP group, revealing a possible brain reserve mechanism of PiL among middle-aged adults.

PiL as a resilience factor in middle-aged individuals

The main result of this investigation was that the association between brain burden and executive performance depends on PiL estimates in middle-aged adults, with subjects with higher PiL exhibiting greater resilience to WMLs. No associations were found regarding episodic or working memory. This aligns with previous literature revealing that WMLs are especially related with executive function and processing speed domains (for a review, see [60]). Furthermore, regarding PiL, previous studies have examined the relationships between this construct and cognitive status, mainly in older samples, with comparable results (i.e., [16, 30, 32]). In a large epidemiologic study of aging, greater sense of purpose was associated with slower rates of cognitive decline and reduced risk of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD [32]. A recent meta-analysis has reinforced these assumptions, revealing that PiL is associated with a reduced risk of dementia [31]. Boyle et al. [34] observed, at the brain level, that superior levels of PiL reduced the deleterious effects of AD pathologic changes on cognitive function. These results suggest that PiL might be a relevant resilience factor, allowing it to counteract the deleterious impact of brain aging and disease on cognitive functions [5, 6]. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study showing that the PiL-dependent association between brain burden and cognition is also present in healthy middle-aged individuals. Therefore, since according to the current operational definitions (https://reserveandresilience.com/), PiL influenced the associations between a measure of brain burden and cognitive status, it can be considered as a key CR factor, reinforcing the notion that resilience can be influenced by multiple genetic and environmental factors operating continuously across the lifespan [5, 6].

Functional brain dynamics underlying PiL

The neurobiological mechanisms explaining the pathways by which PiL might promote resilience are poorly understood. Different explanatory models have been proposed to elucidate how PiL relates to cognitive health (i.e., [61]). It is possible that superior PiL estimates are linked to healthier behaviors [27, 28], which are in turn related to better cognitive performance and lower odds of dementia [62]. Alternatively, PiL might relate to biological mechanisms that influence cognitive performance [63]. Thus, scoring higher in PiL has been reported to be associated with lower interleukin-6 (IL-6) plasma concentration levels [64]. Moreover, in middle-aged [65] and older adults [66], greater PiL was found to be related to lower levels of hemoglobin A1c (HbA1c). Also, Ryff’s Psychological Well-Being Scale measures (including PiL) were examined regarding distinct physiological processes (i.e., blood pressure, urinary catecholamines, and salivary cortisol) in a young and middle-aged sample. In this investigation, data revealed that greater PiL is associated with lower total cortisol levels [67]. This is relevant as inflammation, glucose, and cortisol regulation are associated to cognitive and brain health during the lifespan [68,69,70].

Focusing on brain measures, previous observations have revealed that PiL is linked to a lower risk of cerebrovascular conditions [71, 72], which might confer a resistance role to PiL (i.e., [73]). Furthermore, PiL might help cope with neural damage, providing resilience to brain burden (i.e., [32, 34]). This proposal is consistent with our data, as WMLs were linked to the cognitive status contingent on PiL estimates. Within this context, our study went further on the investigation of the functional brain mechanisms through which PiL may promote cognition, pointing to a particular emphasis on DMN. This aligns with previous investigations that have explored the functional architecture of purpose and sense of life meaning and have revealed a central role of DMN, an archetypical network, which is critically involved in autobiographical remembering, self-referential thought, mental simulation, and mind wandering (i.e., [74,75,76,77]). In this sense, Mwilambwe-Tshilobo et al. [78] found that high levels of meaning in life correlated with increased, and more modular, connectivity between the DMN and the limbic system. Additionally, we also identified key regions within the dDMN associated with superior levels of PiL, even though these topographic results should be further clarified. One of them was the hippocampus, a complex brain structure embedded deep into the temporal lobe. This result links with the observations of Waytz et al. [79], reporting that meaning in life is associated to greater connectivity of the medial temporal lobe subsystem of the DMN.

rs-FC as a mechanism supporting cognition in HP individuals

Overall, while previous findings reinforce the associations between PiL and functional brain measures associated to cognitive function, in our study, we did not found evidence that differences in functional connectivity between the PiL groups, or the associations between HP-related rs-fMRI characteristics and cognitive status, were associated to WMLs burden. These findings therefore indicate that while greater dDMN connectivity among subjects with superior PiL conferred a cognitive advantage on executive functions, this was independent of the deleterious effect of WML in this cognitive domain, and that the brain network’s integrity status was not directly explaining the attenuating effect of high PiL in the relation between WML burden and cognition. Note that these findings differ from those of Ewers et al. [43], where both among familial and sporadic AD, measures of SyS were found to attenuate the effect of pathology (estimated by years to symptom onset in the former and measured by tau-PET in the second) on cognitive function. Within this context, and while our main findings do not challenge the fact that PiL confers resilience to the deleterious effect of WML in middle age, they also suggest that the specific functional mechanism identified here may reflect a brain reserve mechanism (see https://reserveandresilience.com/) but may not involve an active adaptation of functional cognitive processes in the presence of the brain burden measure investigated in the present report.

PiL as a psychological modifiable factor

While the results of the present study highlight that the brain mechanisms through which high PiL confers resilience in middle age need to be investigated in further research, a relevant aspect is that PiL is a potentially modifiable psychological factor. In this context, psychological interventions, such as meaning-centered psychotherapy (MCP), an extension of classic Frankl’s logotherapy further informed by the contributions of Yalom [80], may be implemented to enhance meaning, spiritual well-being, and quality of life [81,82,83,84]. Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) might also help individuals to live meaningful lives by encouraging their engagement in activities that are consistent with their values (i.e., [48]). It is relevant to note that improvements in mental health conditions (i.e., anxiety disorders) from psychological therapy have been shown to be associated with reduced incidence of future dementia [85]. Hence, and as the neurobiological underpinnings of both protective and risk psychological factors (i.e., [19, 86]) are being unveiled, such interventional approaches might help to understand how psychological therapies may promote a healthy brain during the lifespan and aid in the prevention of cognitive impairment later in life.

Limitations

The main limitation of the present study was its cross-sectional nature, which did not allow to explore whether PiL may operate as a resilience mechanism on age-related cognitive decline further than in immediate cognitive status. This also constrains our capacity to infer directionality on the explored variables. Moreover, it is worth highlighting that the use of extreme groups in the present study, as done in previous investigations in the field (i.e., [34]), while allowing the study of PiL concept in a thorough manner, also implies a relevant loss of sample. Finally, part of the neuroimaging results revealing the neurobiological underpinnings of PiL was obtained in an exploratory manner. In particular, the associations between rs-fMRI connectivity measures and cognitive status suggesting a brain reserve effect among PiL needs to be further investigated in forthcoming studies, and in distinct populations (i.e., in the AD continuum).

Conclusion

The present data extend previous findings found in advanced age and pathological aging, such as AD, revealing that having a strong sense of purpose might confer resilience already in middle age. Furthermore, it was also observed that individuals in the HP group had greater inter-network connectivity between specific dDMN nodes, which correlated with cognitive performance. This may represent a possible brain reserve mechanism related to greater PiL, which needs to be further validated.

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding authors K.A.-P and D.B.-F.

Abbreviations

ACT:

Acceptance and commitment therapy

AD:

Alzheimer’s disease

AN:

Attentional network

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

AS:

Anterior salience network

AUD:

Auditory network

BBHI:

Barcelona Brain Health Initiative

BG:

Basal ganglia network

Bnet:

Between-network

BOLD:

Blood-oxygen-level-dependent

CI:

Confidence interval

CR:

Cognitive reserve

CSF:

Cerebrospinal fluid

dDMN:

Dorsal default-mode network

EFc:

Executive functioning composite

EMc:

Episodic memory composite

ETICV:

Estimated total intracranial volume

FOV:

Field of view

FSL:

FMRIB Software Library

FWD:

Framewise displacement

GLM:

General linear model

HbA1c:

Hemoglobin A1c

HP:

Higher purpose in life

hVis:

High visual network

IL-6:

Interleukin-6

LANG:

Language network

LECN:

Left executive control network

LP:

Lower purpose in life

MCI:

Mild cognitive impairment

MCP:

Meaning-centered psychotherapy

midCC:

Midcingulate cortex

MPRAGE:

Magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo

pCC:

Posterior cingulate cortex

PiL:

Purpose in life

PREC:

Precuneus network

prVIS:

Primary visual network

PS:

Posterior salience network

RECN:

Right executive control network

ROI:

Region of interest

rs-FC:

Resting-state functional connectivity

rs-fMRI:

Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging

RSNs:

Resting-state networks

SM:

Sensorimotor network

SPM:

Statistical parametric mapping

SyS:

System segregation

TE:

Echo time

thal:

Thalamus

TR:

Repetition time

vDMN:

Ventral default-mode network

WMc:

Working memory composite

WMHs:

White matter hypointensities

WMLs:

White matter lesions

Wnet:

Within-network

YoE:

Years of education

References

  1. United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division (2020). World Population. Ageing 2019 (ST/ESA/SER.A/444).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Wirth M, Chételat G. Cognitive reserve and lifestyle: moving towards preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:134. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00134 Published 2015 Aug 10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Cattaneo G, Bartrés-Faz D, Morris TP, et al. The Barcelona Brain Health Initiative: a cohort study to define and promote determinants of brain health. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:321. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00321 Published 2018 Oct 11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396(10248):413–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Stern Y, Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Bartrés-Faz D, et al. Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16(9):1305–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Stern Y, Barnes CA, Grady C, Jones RN, Raz N. Brain reserve, cognitive reserve, compensation, and maintenance: operationalization, validity, and mechanisms of cognitive resilience. Neurobiol Aging. 2019;83:124–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.03.022.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Arenaza-Urquijo EM, de Flores R, Gonneaud J, et al. Distinct effects of late adulthood cognitive and physical activities on gray matter volume. Brain Imaging Behav. 2017;11(2):346–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9617-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Casaletto KB, Rentería MA, Pa J, et al. Late-life physical and cognitive activities independently contribute to brain and cognitive resilience. J Alzheimers Dis. 2020;74(1):363–76. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-191114.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Chételat G, Mézenge F, Tomadesso C, et al. Reduced age-associated brain changes in expert meditators: a multimodal neuroimaging pilot study. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):10160. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-07764-x Published 2017 Aug 31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Hernández SE, Dorta R, Suero J, Barros-Loscertales A, González-Mora JL, Rubia K. Larger whole brain grey matter associated with long-term Sahaja Yoga Meditation: a detailed area by area comparison. PloS One. 2020;15(12):e0237552. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237552 Published 2020 Dec 28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Kurth F, Zsadanyi SE, Luders E. Reduced age-related gray matter loss in the subgenual cingulate cortex in long-term meditators. Brain Imaging Behav. 2021;15(6):2824–32. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-021-00578-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Fjell AM, Sørensen Ø, Amlien IK, et al. Poor self-reported sleep is related to regional cortical thinning in aging but not memory decline-results from the Lifebrain Consortium. Cereb Cortex. 2021;31(4):1953–69. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Fjell AM, Sørensen Ø, Amlien IK, et al. Self-reported sleep relates to hippocampal atrophy across the adult lifespan: results from the Lifebrain consortium. Sleep. 2020;43(5):zsz280. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsz280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Anastasiou CA, Yannakoulia M, Kosmidis MH, et al. Mediterranean diet and cognitive health: initial results from the Hellenic Longitudinal Investigation of Ageing and Diet. PloS One. 2017;12(8):e0182048. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182048 Published 2017 Aug 1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Charisis S, Ntanasi E, Yannakoulia M, et al. Mediterranean diet and risk for dementia and cognitive decline in a Mediterranean population. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2021;69(6):1548–59. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.17072.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Bartrés-Faz D, Cattaneo G, Solana J, Tormos JM, Pascual-Leone A. Meaning in life: resilience beyond reserve. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018;10(1):47. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-018-0381-z Published 2018 May 24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Cattaneo G, Solana-Sánchez J, Abellaneda-Pérez K, et al. Sense of coherence mediates the relationship between cognitive reserve and cognition in middle-aged adults. Front Psychol. 2022;13:835415. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.835415 Published 2022 Mar 28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Przybelski SA, Machulda MM, et al. Better stress coping associated with lower tau in amyloid-positive cognitively unimpaired older adults. Neurology. 2020;94(15):e1571–9. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000008979.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Marchant NL, Lovland LR, Jones R, et al. Repetitive negative thinking is associated with amyloid, tau, and cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16(7):1054–64. https://doi.org/10.1002/alz.12116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ryff CD, Keyes CLM. The structure of psychological well-being revisited. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(4):719–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Ryff CD. Beyond Ponce de Leon and Life Satisfaction: new directions in quest of successful ageing. Int J Behav Dev. 1989;12(1):35–55. https://doi.org/10.1177/016502548901200102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Ryff CD. Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;57(6):1069–81. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Boyle PA, Barnes LL, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Purpose in life is associated with mortality among community-dwelling older persons. Psychosom Med. 2009;71(5):574–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181a5a7c0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Hicks JA, Routledge C. The experience of meaning in life. Netherlands: Springer; 2013. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-6527-6.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  25. Ryff CD, Heller AS, Schaefer SM, van Reekum C, Davidson RJ. Purposeful engagement, healthy aging, and the brain. Curr Behav Neurosci Rep. 2016;3(4):318–27. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40473-016-0096-z.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Ryff CD. Psychological well-being revisited: advances in the science and practice of eudaimonia. Psychother Psychosom. 2014;83(1):10–28. https://doi.org/10.1159/000353263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Kim ES, Shiba K, Boehm JK, Kubzansky LD. Sense of purpose in life and five health behaviors in older adults. Prev Med. 2020;139:106172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2020.106172.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Kim ES, Strecher VJ, Ryff CD. Purpose in life and use of preventive health care services. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(46):16331–6. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1414826111.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Alimujiang A, Wiensch A, Boss J, et al. Association between life purpose and mortality among US adults older than 50 years. JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(5):e194270. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.4270 Published 2019 May 3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Lewis NA, Turiano NA, Payne BR, Hill PL. Purpose in life and cognitive functioning in adulthood. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2017;24(6):662–71. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2016.1251549.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Bell G, Singham T, Saunders R, John A, Stott J. Positive psychological constructs and association with reduced risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2022;77:101594. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2022.101594.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Barnes LL, Bennett DA. Effect of a purpose in life on risk of incident Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older persons. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(3):304–10. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.208.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Purpose in life is associated with a reduced risk of incident disability among community-dwelling older persons. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(12):1093–102. https://doi.org/10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181d6c259.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Wilson RS, Yu L, Schneider JA, Bennett DA. Effect of purpose in life on the relation between Alzheimer disease pathologic changes on cognitive function in advanced age. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(5):499–505. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1487.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Okuzono SS, Shiba K, Kim ES, et al. Ikigai and subsequent health and wellbeing among Japanese older adults: longitudinal outcome-wide analysis. Lancet Reg Health West Pac. 2022;21:100391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lanwpc.2022.100391 Published 2022 Feb 3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Tanno K, Sakata K, Ohsawa M, et al. Associations of ikigai as a positive psychological factor with all-cause mortality and cause-specific mortality among middle-aged and elderly Japanese people: findings from the Japan Collaborative Cohort Study. J Psychosom Res. 2009;67(1):67–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.10.018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. de Leeuw FE, de Groot JC, Oudkerk M, et al. Hypertension and cerebral white matter lesions in a prospective cohort study. Brain. 2002;125(Pt 4):765–72. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf077.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Habes M, Erus G, Toledo JB, et al. Regional tract-specific white matter hyperintensities are associated with patterns to aging-related brain atrophy via vascular risk factors, but also independently. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2018;10:278–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2018.02.002 Published 2018 Mar 5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Habes M, Erus G, Toledo JB, et al. White matter hyperintensities and imaging patterns of brain ageing in the general population. Brain. 2016;139(Pt 4):1164–79. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aww008.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Vernooij MW, Ikram MA, Tanghe HL, et al. Incidental findings on brain MRI in the general population. N Engl J Med. 2007;357(18):1821–8. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa070972.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Wig GS. Segregated systems of human brain networks. Trends Cogn Sci. 2017;21(12):981–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.09.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Chan MY, Park DC, Savalia NK, Petersen SE, Wig GS. Decreased segregation of brain systems across the healthy adult lifespan. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(46):E4997–5006. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1415122111.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Ewers M, Luan Y, Frontzkowski L, et al. Segregation of functional networks is associated with cognitive resilience in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2021;144(7):2176–85. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awab112.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Arfanakis K, Evia AM, Leurgans SE, et al. Neuropathologic correlates of white matter hyperintensities in a community-based cohort of older adults. J Alzheimers Dis. 2020;73(1):333–45. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-190687.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Walsh P, Sudre CH, Fiford CM, et al. CSF amyloid is a consistent predictor of white matter hyperintensities across the disease course from aging to Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2020;91:5–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2020.03.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Black S, Gao F, Bilbao J. Understanding white matter disease: imaging-pathological correlations in vascular cognitive impairment. Stroke. 2009;40(3 Suppl):S48–52. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.537704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Vasquez BP, Zakzanis KK. The neuropsychological profile of vascular cognitive impairment not demented: a meta-analysis. J Neuropsychol. 2015;9(1):109–36. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12039.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Bai Z, Luo S, Zhang L, Wu S, Chi I. Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) to reduce depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2020;260:728–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.09.040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Cattaneo G, Bartrés-Faz D, Morris TP, et al. The Barcelona Brain Health Initiative: cohort description and first follow-up. PloS One. 2020;15(2):e0228754. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228754 Published 2020 Feb 11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Razali NM, Wah YB. Power comparisons of Shapiro-Wilk, Kolmogorov-Smirnov, Lilliefors and Anderson-Darling tests. J Stat Model Anal. 2011;2(1):21–33.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Shapiro SS, Wilk MB. An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika. 1965;52(3/4):591. https://doi.org/10.2307/2333709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Peña-Casanova J, Casals-Coll M, Quintana M, et al. Estudios normativos españoles en población adulta joven (Proyecto NEURONORMA jóvenes): métodos y características de la muestra. Neurología. 2012;27(5):253–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nrl.2011.12.019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale--fourth edition. PsycTESTS Dataset. Published online 2008. https://doi.org/10.1037/t15169-000

  54. Bowler D. Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Rey AVLT). In: Volkmar FR, editor. Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders. New York: Springer; 2013. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1698-3_539.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  55. Wei K, Tran T, Chu K, et al. White matter hypointensities and hyperintensities have equivalent correlations with age and CSF β-amyloid in the nondemented elderly. Brain Behav. 2019;9(12):e01457. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.1457.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Cedres N, Ferreira D, Machado A, et al. Predicting Fazekas scores from automatic segmentations of white matter signal abnormalities. Aging (Albany NY). 2020;12(1):894–901. https://doi.org/10.18632/aging.102662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Al-Janabi OM, Bauer CE, Goldstein LB, et al. White matter hyperintensity regression: comparison of brain atrophy and cognitive profiles with progression and stable groups. Brain Sci. 2019;9(7):170. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9070170 Published 2019 Jul 19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Gutiérrez-Zúñiga R, Diez I, Bueichekú E, et al. Connectomic-genetic signatures in the cerebral small vessel disease. Neurobiol Dis. 2022;167:105671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nbd.2022.105671.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Shirer WR, Ryali S, Rykhlevskaia E, Menon V, Greicius MD. Decoding subject-driven cognitive states with whole-brain connectivity patterns. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22(1):158–65. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr099.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Debette S, Markus HS. The clinical importance of white matter hyperintensities on brain magnetic resonance imaging: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ. 2010;341:c3666. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.c3666 Published 2010 Jul 26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Wagner M, Guimond AJ, Kubzansky LD, et al. Negative and positive psychosocial factors in relation to cognitive health in older African Americans. Innov. Aging. 2022;6(3):igac019. https://doi.org/10.1093/geroni/igac019 Published 2022 Apr 1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Kivipelto M, Mangialasche F, Ngandu T. Lifestyle interventions to prevent cognitive impairment, dementia and Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurol. 2018;14(11):653–66. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41582-018-0070-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Ryff CD, Dienberg Love G, Urry HL, et al. Psychological well-being and ill-being: do they have distinct or mirrored biological correlates? Psychother Psychosom. 2006;75(2):85–95. https://doi.org/10.1159/000090892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Friedman EM, Hayney M, Love GD, Singer BH, Ryff CD. Plasma interleukin-6 and soluble IL-6 receptors are associated with psychological well-being in aging women. Health Psychol. 2007;26(3):305–13. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.26.3.305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Boylan JM, Tsenkova VK, Miyamoto Y, Ryff CD. Psychological resources and glucoregulation in Japanese adults: findings from MIDJA. Health Psychol. 2017;36(5):449–57. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000455.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Hafez D, Heisler M, Choi H, Ankuda CK, Winkelman T, Kullgren JT. Association between purpose in life and glucose control among older adults. Ann Behav Med. 2018;52(4):309–18. https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/kax012.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Lindfors P, Lundberg U. Is low cortisol release an indicator of positive health? Stress Health. 2002;18(4):153–60. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Darweesh SKL, Wolters FJ, Ikram MA, de Wolf F, Bos D, Hofman A. Inflammatory markers and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(11):1450–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Echouffo-Tcheugui JB, Conner SC, Himali JJ, et al. Circulating cortisol and cognitive and structural brain measures: the Framingham Heart Study. Neurology. 2018;91(21):e1961–70. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000006549.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Yaffe K, Lindquist K, Penninx BW, et al. Inflammatory markers and cognition in well-functioning African-American and white elders. Neurology. 2003;61(1):76–80. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000073620.42047.d7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Kim ES, Sun JK, Park N, Kubzansky LD, Peterson C. Purpose in life and reduced risk of myocardial infarction among older U.S. adults with coronary heart disease: a two-year follow-up. J Behav Med. 2013;36(2):124–33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-012-9406-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Yu L, Boyle PA, Wilson RS, Levine SR, Schneider JA, Bennett DA. Purpose in life and cerebral infarcts in community-dwelling older people. Stroke. 2015;46(4):1071–6. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.008010.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Vemuri P. Resistance vs resilience to Alzheimer disease: clarifying terminology for preclinical studies. Neurology. 2018;90(15):695–703. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000005303.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Alves PN, Foulon C, Karolis V, et al. An improved neuroanatomical model of the default-mode network reconciles previous neuroimaging and neuropathological findings. Commun Biol. 2019;2:370. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-019-0611-3 Published 2019 Oct 10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Andrews-Hanna JR, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, et al. Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron. 2007;56(5):924–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.038.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:1–38. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Davey CG, Pujol J, Harrison BJ. Mapping the self in the brain’s default mode network. Neuroimage. 2016;132:390–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Mwilambwe-Tshilobo L, Ge T, Chong M, et al. Loneliness and meaning in life are reflected in the intrinsic network architecture of the brain. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2019;14(4):423–33. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsz021.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Waytz A, Hershfield HE, Tamir DI. Mental simulation and meaning in life. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2015;108(2):336–55. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038322.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Yalom ID. Existential psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books; 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  81. Breitbart W, Pessin H, Rosenfeld B, et al. Individual meaning-centered psychotherapy for the treatment of psychological and existential distress: a randomized controlled trial in patients with advanced cancer. Cancer. 2018;124(15):3231–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/cncr.31539.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Breitbart W, Rosenfeld B, Pessin H, Applebaum A, Kulikowski J, Lichtenthal WG. Meaning-centered group psychotherapy: an effective intervention for improving psychological well-being in patients with advanced cancer. J Clin Oncol. 2015;33(7):749–54. https://doi.org/10.1200/JCO.2014.57.2198.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Thomas LP, Meier EA, Irwin SA. Meaning-centered psychotherapy: a form of psychotherapy for patients with cancer. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2014;16(10):488. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-014-0488-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Vos J, Vitali D. The effects of psychological meaning-centered therapies on quality of life and psychological stress: a metaanalysis. Palliat Support Care. 2018;16(5):608–32. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1478951517000931.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Stott J, Saunders R, Desai R, et al. Associations between psychological intervention for anxiety disorders and risk of dementia: a prospective cohort study using national health-care records data in England [published online ahead of print, 2022 Dec 9]. Lancet Healthy Longev. 2022;S2666-7568(22):00242-2. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2666-7568(22)00242-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Solé-Padullés C, Cattaneo G, Marchant NL, et al. Associations between repetitive negative thinking and resting-state network segregation among healthy middle-aged adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:1062887. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2022.1062887 Published 2022 Dec 15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The research leading to these results has received funding from the “la Caixa” Foundation (grant agreement no. LCF/PR/PR16/11110004). This study is also partly supported by a grant from La Marató de TV3 MARATÓ 2020 COVID-19 (Grant No. 202129-31) and grants from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (RTI2018-095181-B-C2) and the National Institutes of Health (R24AG06142, and P01AG031720). K.A.-P. was financially supported by a Juan de la Cierva-Formación research grant (FJC2021-047380-I) of the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation. L.V.-A. was supported by a Margarita Salas junior postdoctoral fellowship (UNI/551/2021, NextGenerationUE). R.P.-A. was supported by a fellowship from the “la Caixa” Foundation (ID 100010434; Fellowship code: LCF/BQ/DI19/11730050). D.B-F was supported by an Institut Català de Recerca i Estudis Avançats, ICREA Academia 2019 award from the Catalan government.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

K.A.-P.—conceptualization, data analyses, writing, review, and editing. G.C.—conceptualization, database management and curation, writing, review, and editing. M.R.C.-T.—database management and curation, neuroimaging data computation, review, and editing. J.S.—conceptualization, database management and curation, review, and editing. L.M.-P.—data acquisition, database management and curation, review, and editing. L.V.-A.—database management, neuroimaging data supervision, review, and editing. R.P.-A.—data analyses, review, and editing. C.S.-P.—review and editing. N.B.—neuroimaging data supervision, review, and editing. J.M.T.—conceptualization, supervision, review, and editing. A.P.-L.—conceptualization, supervision, review, and editing. D.B.-F.—conceptualization, supervision, review, and editing. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Kilian Abellaneda-Pérez or David Bartrés-Faz.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All participants gave written informed consent according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki, and the study protocols were approved by the Comitè Ètic d’Investigació (CEIm) de la Fundació Unió Catalana d’Hospitals (ref. CEIC 17/06).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

A.P.-L. is listed as an inventor on several issued and pending patents on the real-time integration of transcranial magnetic stimulation with electroencephalography and magnetic resonance imaging. He is a co-founder of Linus Health and TI Solutions AG and serves on the scientific advisory boards for Starlab Neuroscience, Magstim Inc., Hearts Radiant, MedRhythms, TetraNeuron, and Skin2Neuron.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Demographic data in the higher (HP) and lower purpose in life (LP) groups and differences between them using ANOVAa and chi-squared testsb. Abbreviations: Diff: Differences, M: Men, PiL: Purpose in life, SD: Standard deviation, W: Women, YoE: Years of education. Table S2. dDMN functional connections observed when comparing HP vs. LP groups. Data presented has been obtained through GLM analyses. Abbreviations: AN: Attentional network, ang: Angular gyrus, AUD: Auditory network, calcar: Calcarine sulcus, dDMN: Dorsal default-mode network, hipp: Hippocampus, hVis: High visual network, infPar: Inferior parietal sulcus, infTemp: Inferior temporal gyrus, L: Left, LECN: Left executive-control network, medPref-ACC-orb: Medial prefrontal cortex - anterior cingulate cortex - orbitofrontal cortex, midCC-pCC: Midcingulate cortex - posterior cingulate cortex, midCC: Midcingulate cortex, midFront: Middle frontal gyrus, midOcc-supOcc: Middle occipital gyrus, superior occipital gyrus, midTemp: Middle temporal gyrus, postIns-put: Posterior insula – putamen, PREC: Precuneus network, prec: Precuneus, precen-postcen: Precentral gyrus – postcentral gyrus, prVIS: primary visual network, PS: Posterior salience network, R: Right, RECN: Right executive-control network, SM: sensorimotor network, sma: Supplementary motor area, supFront-midFront: Superior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, supFront: Superior frontal gyrus, supPar-prec: Superior parietal gyrus – precuneus, supramar-infPar: Supramarginal gyrus – inferior parietal gyrus, supTemp-hesc: Superior temporal gyrus - Heschl’s Gyrus, supTemp: Superior temporal gyrus, thal: Thalamus, vDMN: Ventral default-mode network.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Abellaneda-Pérez, K., Cattaneo, G., Cabello-Toscano, M. et al. Purpose in life promotes resilience to age-related brain burden in middle-aged adults. Alz Res Therapy 15, 49 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-023-01198-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-023-01198-6

Keywords